SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

Keywords:

  • emotional eating;
  • binge eating;
  • ovarian hormones;
  • menstrual cycle;
  • body mass index;
  • dietary restraint

Abstract

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Introduction
  4. Method
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. References

Objective:

Associations between within-person changes in ovarian hormones and dysregulated eating (binge eating, emotional eating) have been observed across the menstrual cycle. However, studies have not examined moderators that may contribute to differential associations between individuals. We investigated body-weight regulation variables [body mass index (BMI), dietary restraint] that have theoretical relevance by virtue of their associations with both phenotypes.

Method:

Women (N = 196) provided emotional eating ratings and saliva samples for 45 days. BMI and restraint were assessed at three time points and averaged.

Results:

Results showed significant estradiol × progesterone interactions in the prediction of within-subject changes in emotional eating. Neither BMI nor restraint moderated these relationships, although a trend-level dietary restraint × estradiol interaction was observed where estradiol's effects were enhanced in high restraint scorers.

Discussion:

Findings confirm a role for hormones in changes in emotional eating and suggest that restraint might enhance hormone effects in severegroups. © 2013 by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. (Int J Eat Disord 2013)


Introduction

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Introduction
  4. Method
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. References

Emerging data suggest significant associations between within-person changes in ovarian hormones and changes in dysregulated eating across the menstrual cycle. Binge eating and emotional eating reach their peak during the midluteal phase of the cycle1–4 and are predicted by within-person changes in estrogen and progesterone.1–3 Early work suggested that low levels of estradiol, and high levels of progesterone, predict increased binge eating across the menstrual cycle in women with bulimia nervosa (BN)1, 4 and from the community.2, 3 However, these studies did not examine interactions between estradiol and progesterone, despite animal studies demonstrating significant interactions between the hormones (with progesterone antagonizing the effects of estrogen) for food intake.5, 6

In the largest sample of women studied to date, our group found significant interactions between estrogen and progesterone, where emotional eating scores were highest when both estradiol and progesterone levels were elevated.3 These interactions seemed to account for midluteal increases in dysregulated eating, as progesterone reaches its peak during the midluteal phase and estrogen exhibits a secondary peak as well. Importantly, we accounted for within-person, menstrual cycle changes in body mass index (BMI) and negative affect in all analyses; although changes in BMI and negative affect significantly predicted changes in emotional eating across the menstrual cycle, changes in hormones remained significant predictors of emotional eating, even after controlling for the effects of these covariates. Finally, the direction of associations was from the changes in hormones to the changes in emotional eating (rather than the reverse).3 Data from our studies2, 3 and those of others1, 4 suggest that these within-person hormone/dysregulated eating associations are present across the spectrum of binge eating tendencies in women with BN1, 4 and women from the community.2, 3

Although these studies have been helpful for identifying female-specific, biological risk factors for changes in dysregulated eating across the menstrual cycle, they have not identified specific conditions under which hormonal changes increase risk for dysregulated eating. Identifying potential, between-subject moderators of hormone/dysregulated eating relationships could significantly increase understanding of the multitude of factors contributing to menstrual cycle changes in dysregulated eating.

A particularly important set of moderators to examine are those related to body weight regulation, including BMI and dietary restraint (i.e., behavioral and/or cognitive attempts to restrict food intake). Both risk factors predict the development and maintenance of dysregulated eating (i.e., binge eating, emotional eating). For example, higher BMI and higher dietary restraint scores predict binge eating7–12 and emotional eating.13–18 Although dietary restraint measures were originally conceptualized as assessing actual dieting behaviors (e.g., decreased caloric content), recent research indicates that self-reported dietary restraint may capture an “intent to diet” rather than pure caloric restriction per se, as actual food intake does not always map on to self-reports of dietary restraint.19, 20

Importantly, although within-subject hormone/dysregulated eating associations cannot be accounted for by changes in BMI across the menstrual cycle (i.e., they are not mediated by BMI changes; see above), it remains unknown whether between-subject differences in BMI and/or dietary restraint change the magnitude of associations between within-person changes in hormones and dysregulated eating. These variables could strengthen associations between within-person changes in hormones and dysregulated eating by increasing the risk for dysregulated eating in the presence of vulnerable hormone environments.

Alternatively, it is possible that these between-subject variables could attenuate hormone/dysregulated eating associations through their effects on ovarian hormone levels. Higher BMI has been associated with elevated hormone levels (particularly estrogen),21, 22 whereas lower BMI and sustained dieting can lead to decreased hormone levels.23 Both types of effects attenuate within-person changes in hormones across the menstrual cycle,23, 24 as they can lead to chronically high or low hormone levels, regardless of menstrual cycle phase. In that case, we also would expect attenuated changes in dysregulated eating and decreased associations between within-person changes in both variables.

To address both of these possibilities, we examined the moderating effects of BMI and dietary restraint on associations between within-person changes in hormones and emotional eating across the menstrual cycle in the large, community-based sample of women described in Klump et al.3 As in our previous analyses, we controlled for within-subject changes in negative affect in all analyses, as negative affect is a potent predictor of within-person changes in emotional eating25 and is also related to both of the moderators examined herein.

Method

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Introduction
  4. Method
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. References

Participants

Participants were 196 (110 monozygotic (MZ), of which 86 dizygotic (DZ)) female twins ages 16 to 22 years drawn from the “Twin Study of Hormones and Behavior across the Menstrual Cycle” project from the Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR; N ∼ 19,000 twins).26 The MSUTR recruits twins through birth records using methods described previously.26 Prior27 and current analyses indicate that MSUTR twins are demographically representative of the recruitment region (89% Caucasian; 11% African American).

Inclusion/exclusion criteria ensured that we captured natural variations in hormones: (1) menstruation every 22–32 days for the past 6 months; (2) no hormonal contraceptive use in the past 3 months; (3) no psychotropic or steroid medications within the past 4 weeks; (4) no pregnancy or lactation within the past 6 months; and (5) no history of genetic or medical conditions known to influence hormone functioning or appetite/weight. Despite these criteria, BMI, emotional eating scores, and restraint scores did not differ between our participants and other MSUTR twins (d's =. 02–.18).

Procedures

The larger data collection included behavioral and hormone measures that were collected daily across a 45-day study period, as well as repeated measures that were collected during three in-person study visits. The daily measures included ratings of emotional eating, negative affect, and the collection of salivary samples for ovarian hormones. Salivary samples were collected every morning within 30 min of waking (see Klump et al.2) for a detailed description of methods), while daily ratings of emotional eating and negative affect were made each evening (after 5:00 pm). The timing of data collection ensured that a given day's hormone collection preceded that day's behavioral ratings.

In addition to the daily measures, three in-person visits occurred at the start of the study (i.e., “intake” session), midway through data collection (∼day 23, “intermediate” session), and at the end of data collection (∼day 45, “final” session). Each in-person visit included assessments of BMI and dietary restraint (see below) as well as a re-assessment of study eligibility and the collection of all samples. Between visits, staff contacted participants 1×/week to answer questions and confirm protocol adherence. These procedures were effective for minimizing dropouts (3%) and missing data (≤6%) and identifying twins who were no longer eligible (3% due to pregnancy or medication use).

Measures

Sample characteristics on all study measures are presented in Table 1. Although we used daily data for emotional eating, hormones, and negative affect, we present average values across the data collection period for all variables to characterize the sample and allow for comparisons of sample composition in future work.

Table 1. Sample Characteristics
VariablesMean (SD)Range in Sample
  1. Notes: Values for BMI and dietary restraint are average values across the three in-person assessments. Values for emotional eating, estradiol, progesterone, and negative affect are average values across the 45-day data collection period.

Age17.84 (1.58)16–22
Body mass index23.98 (5.63)16.71–46.78
Dietary restraint0.57 (0.82)0–4
Emotional eating0.34 (0.43)0–3
Estradiol (pg/mL)3.19 (0.03)0.95–10.63
Progesterone (pg/mL)119.05 (63.21)22.41–368.00
Negative affect15.09 (4.06)10–35
Daily Measures of Emotional Eating, Hormones, and Negative Affect
Emotional Eating

Emotional eating was assessed with the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ).17 The DEBQ emotional eating subscale assesses the tendency to eat excessive amounts of food for reasons that are typically endorsed by individuals who experience recurrent binge-eating episodes (e.g., “Did you have a desire to eat when you were discouraged?”). This scale was used in the previous community study of ovarian hormones/binge eating,2 because it exhibits robust fluctuations across the menstrual cycle2 and is significantly correlated with traditional binge measures (e.g., r's =. 55–.69)28, 29 as well as palatable food intake (e.g., ice cream) in the laboratory.30 Subscale scores also successfully discriminate among normal controls, overweight women, and patients with bulimia nervosa.31 Similar to previous research,2 instructions were modified with permission to refer to that day (i.e., “true in relation to you TODAY”). Internal consistency of our modified version was excellent (average α =. 90).

Hormones

Hormone concentrations were measured in saliva. Saliva collection is a noninvasive method for repeated sampling schedules that is associated with higher compliance and more robust hormone-behavior associations than bloodspots.1

Salivary assays were conducted by Salimetrics, LLC (State College, PA) using enzyme immunoassay kits that show excellent intra-assay and inter-assay coefficients of variation (estradiol = 7.1 and 7.5%; progesterone = 6.2 and 7.6%), assay sensitivity (determined by interpolating the mean optical density minus 2 SDs of 10–20 replicates at the 0 pg/mL level; estradiol = 0.10 pg/mL; progesterone = 5 pg/mL) and method accuracy (determined by spike recovery and linearity; estradiol = 104.2 and 99.4%; progesterone = 99.6 and 91.8%). To capture key periods of hormonal change (e.g., mid-late follicular through the premenstrual phase) while maximizing the number of participants assessed, we assayed samples every other day during menstrual bleeding and the early follicular phase when hormone levels were expected to be low and stable.

Negative Affect

The Negative Affect scale from the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule32 was used to measure negative affect (i.e., depression and anxiety) each day. This scale exhibits good convergent and discriminant validity32 and excellent internal consistency (average α =. 85 in our sample).

BMI and Dietary Restraint

As noted above, BMI and dietary restraint were collected at the intake, intermediate, and final in-person assessment sessions only. Because we were interested in examining how stable, between-subject differences in these variables could moderate within-person associations between hormones and emotional eating, we calculated the average BMI and average dietary restraint score across the three assessment periods and used these trait levels in analyses. This approach was supported by high correlations across the three assessment periods for both variables (BMI r's >. 99; dietary restraint r's =. 70–.81).

Body Mass Index

BMI [weight (in kilograms)/height (in meters)2] was calculated using height and weight measured with a wall-mounted ruler and digital scale, respectively.

Dietary Restraint

The Restraint subscale from the Eating Disorders Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q)33 was used to assess dietary restraint in all participants. This subscale assesses attempt to restrain eating, regardless of actual success, and has demonstrated excellent internal consistency33–35 and convergent validity with the restraint subscale of the EDE interview.33, 35, 36

Statistical Analyses

Data Preparation

We standardized our between-subject moderators (i.e., BMI, dietary restraint score) by the overall sample mean and standard deviation prior to analysis to aid in interpretation of moderation effects. Similar to past studies1, 2 and our previous analysis of this dataset,3 we calculated five-day rolling averages for the within-subject, daily measures (i.e., hormones, emotional eating, and negative affect) to reduce the influence of hormone pulsatility and increase the signal-to-noise ratio.37–39 These averages were then converted into within-person Z scores based upon each participant's overall mean and standard deviation across data collection. Within-person, standardized values are ideal because they index the extent to which changes in a woman's hormones, relative to her equilibrium, predict changes away from her equilibrium in emotional eating.1, 2, 4, 37 They also tend to be sensitive to small deviations from equilibria, making them ideal indicators of how within-person changes in hormones predict changes in emotional eating in nonclinical samples of women.

Missing daily data were handled two ways. We prorated raw scores for all behavioral measures (e.g., emotional eating, dietary restraint, and negative affect) when ≤10% of the items were missing, and coded the scores as missing if >10% of items were missing. For the daily measures data (e.g., hormones, emotional eating, negative affect), we calculated the rolling averages if ≥3 days of data were available within the 5-day window. If fewer than 3 days were available, then the average for that day was coded as missing (occurring <2.6% of the time).

Moderation

We used mixed linear models (MLMs) to examine the extent to which average BMI and dietary restraint moderated associations between within-subject changes in hormones and emotional eating across the menstrual cycle. MLMs are ideal for these analyses, as we can examine moderating effects while controlling for negative affect and the nonindependence of the repeated measures and twin data. We controlled for the nonindependence of the twin data by estimating random intercepts and allowing them to correlatea. Because most cotwins were assessed across the same 45-day period, we also estimated a time-specific dyadic correlation that allowed the twins' residual scores for emotional eating to correlate from day to day. For each of these random effects, we specified a compound symmetry covariance structure, which estimates a single intercept variance or a single residual variance across twins and time. The models also allowed for random slopes for each of the repeated measures predictors (i.e., hormones, negative affect). However, because there was no evidence that these slopes correlated across twins, we fixed the cross-twin correlation to zero for all predictors.

Because we were interested in average BMI and dietary restraint as between-subject moderators of hormone/emotional eating associations, our models focused on testing the main effects of estradiol and progesterone, as well as all two-way (i.e., moderator × estradiol; moderator × progesterone, estradiol × progesterone) and three-way (moderator × estradiol × progesterone) interactions. A significant two- or three-way interaction between the moderator and hormone levels would indicate that hormone-behavior associations differ according to the moderator level (e.g., that hormone-behavior associations are stronger in individuals with higher BMIs).

Results

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Introduction
  4. Method
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. References

As shown in Table 2, the strongest predictor of emotional eating scores was the estradiol × progesterone interaction. The nature of this interaction in both models (see Fig. 1a for an example in the dietary restraint model) was nearly identical to that observed in our prior analysis where higher levels of both estradiol and progesterone (which characterize the midluteal phase of the menstrual cycle) were associated with increased emotional eating.3

thumbnail image

Figure 1.. Interactions between estradiol and progesterone, and estradiol and dietary restraint, in the dietary restraint model. “Emotional Eating Z score” = 5-day rolling average calculated within participants, then averaged across participants.

Download figure to PowerPoint

Table 2. The moderating effects of body mass index (BMI) and dietary restraint on within-person associations between ovarian hormones and emotional eating
Modelsb (SE)tdfp
Body max index (BMI) as a moderator
 Intercept−.04 (.01)−3.39177.001
 Estradiol−.01 (.02)−0.63215.53
 Progesterone.04 (.02)1.77213.08
 Negative affect.18 (.03)7.17211<.001
 BMI.01 (.01)0.83176.41
 BMI × estradiol.004 (.02)0.16211.87
 BMI × progesterone−.02 (.02)−1.10210.27
 Estradiol × progesterone.05 (.02)2.85202.005
 BMI × estradiol × progesterone.008 (.02)0.44179.66
Dietary restraint as a moderator
 Intercept−.04 (.01)−3.31161.001
 Estradiol−.004 (.02)−.15193.88
 Progesterone.04 (.02)1.85193.07
 Negative affect.17 (.02)6.54191<.001
 Restraint.02 (.01)1.18160.24
 Restraint × estradiol−.04 (.02)−1.88193.07
 Restraint × progesterone.02 (.02)1.09185.28
 Estradiol × progesterone.05 (.02)2.53185.012
 Restraint × estradiol × progesterone.005 (.02)0.26205.79

Importantly, the main effects of average BMI and restraint, and the two- and three-way interactions between the moderators and ovarian hormones, were all non-significant. These findings indicate that: (1) unlike within-subject changes in BMI (see Introduction and Klump et al., in press), between-subject differences in BMI and dietary restraint do not significantly predict within-subject changes in emotional eating across the menstrual cycle; and, most importantly (2) between-subject differences in BMI and levels of dietary restraint also do not affect the strength of associations between within-subject changes in ovarian hormones and emotional eating across the menstrual cycleb.

However, there was a trend-level interaction (p =. 07) between dietary restraint and estradiol, where the highest levels of emotional eating were observed at low estradiol levels (see Fig. 1b). This interaction may seem contradictory or at odds with the significant estradiol × progesterone interaction, as one interaction suggests that emotional eating is highest at low estradiol levels (dependent on dietary restraint), whereas the other suggests that emotional eating is highest at high estradiol levels (dependent on progesterone). These findings can be reconciled when one considers the context for the changes, that is, the menstrual cycle. Figure 2 includes trajectories of emotional eating, estradiol, and progesterone across menstrual cycle phase (i.e., follicular, ovulatory, midluteal, and premenstrual) in women who scored one standard deviation below (N = 51; see Fig. 2a) or above (N = 25; see Fig. 2b) the mean dietary restraint score in the sample. Similar to previous studies,1–4 peaks in emotional eating scores were observed in the midluteal phase and, to lesser extent, in the follicular phase in low restraint scorers. This pattern maps on well to the estradiol × progesterone interaction in Figure 1, as the midluteal phase is characterized by the highest levels of progesterone and the second highest levels of estradiol of the cycle.

thumbnail image

Figure 2.. Trajectories of Emotional Eating scores, estradiol levels, and progesterone levels across menstrual cycle phases in scorers falling: (a) one standard deviation below the dietary restraint mean; and (b) one standard deviation above the dietary restraint mean. Mean Z score = the mean of the 5-day rolling averages calculated within participants, then averaged across participants. The number of days included in each phase varied by participant based on their cycle length, but the days roughly corresponded to the following (first day of menstrual bleeding = +1; previous day = −1): follicular = +3 to +12; ovulatory = −15 to −12; midluteal = −9 to −5; premenstrual = −3 to +1.

Download figure to PowerPoint

By contrast, in the high restraint group, emotional eating scores were elevated during both the midluteal and the premenstrual phase. The premenstrual phase is characterized by low levels of estradiol and progesterone and has been shown in previous work to be associated with higher levels of binge eating in women with BN.1, 4, 37 Our findings suggest that elevated levels of dysregulated eating during the premenstrual phase in some (i.e., samples with BN) but not all (e.g., unselected, community samples) samples may be due to increased levels of dietary restraint and the interaction between restraint and low levels of estradiol. Given that progesterone tends to be positively associated with emotional eating scores, it is not surprising that no significant interactions were observed between restraint and progesterone, as progesterone levels are low during the premenstrual phase.

Discussion

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Introduction
  4. Method
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. References

Our findings extend previous work by examining moderators of associations between within-person changes in ovarian hormones and emotional eating across the menstrual cycle. Overall, results suggest that hormone/dysregulated eating associations are robust to between-subject differences in trait levels of BMI and dietary restraint, as analyses showed no significant interactions between these variables and ovarian hormones. Our prior analyses had suggested that hormone/behavior associations were not mediated by changes in body weight across the menstrual cycle (i.e., they were not due to these body weight changes);3 our current results extend these findings by showing that between-subject differences in BMI and levels of dietary restraint also do not moderate the strength of hormone/binge eating associations. The fact that the estrogen × progesterone interaction remained significant in the presence of other explanatory variables highlights the importance of ovarian hormones for changes in emotional eating across the menstrual cycle.

There was one minor exception to this pattern of findings—dietary restraint exhibited a trend-level interaction with estradiol, such that emotional eating scores were highest when dietary restraint was high and estradiol levels were low. Importantly, the three-way interaction between restraint and both hormones was nonsignificant, suggesting that these effects were independent of significant interactions between estradiol and progesterone. Graphs of hormone/emotional eating interactions (Figs. 1 and 2) and trajectories across the menstrual cycle suggested that the estradiol × progesterone interaction contributed to elevated emotional eating in all participants, whereas the trend-level restraint × estradiol interaction contributed to elevated emotional eating scores during the premenstrual phase in high restraint scorers only.

Overall, our findings add to a growing literature aiming to understand within-person changes in dysregulated eating across the menstrual cycle. In community samples of women, who exhibit the full range of variation in emotional eating, body weight, and dietary restraint, the strongest predictors of within-subject changes in dysregulated eating across the menstrual cycle are changes in ovarian hormones, most specifically, interactions between estradiol and progesterone.2 In these women, the primary force driving increased emotional eating during the midluteal phase appears to be progesterone's antagonizing effects on estrogen, that is, progesterone removes the inhibitory effects of estradiol on food intake.

In more disturbed samples (i.e., those with higher dietary restraint scores), we observe broader and potentially stronger1–3 hormone effects on dysregulated eating where both estradiol × progesterone interactions and, to a lesser extent, estradiol × restraint interactions contribute to within-subject changes in emotional eating across different menstrual cycle phases. Reasons for the novel effects of estradiol on emotional eating in high restraint scorers remain unclear, but likely relate to processes described earlier, i.e., high dietary restraint (a potent risk factor for dysregulated eating) increases risk for emotional eating in the presence of a vulnerable hormone environment (i.e., low estradiol levels). Because we did not collect food records in this study, it is unknown whether high restraint scores reflect decreased caloric intake and/or “intent to diet”/cognitive restraint. Regardless of the exact processes, however, it appears that emotional eating is more likely to occur in high restraint women when estradiol and progesterone levels are high during the midluteal phase and when their cognitive/behavioral control is disrupted by low estradiol levels and associated increased craving/hunger for food (particularly palatable food)23, 40 during the premenstrual phase.

Although we feel that our study had many strengths (e.g., large sample size, daily measurements of emotional eating and hormones), there are two key limitations. First, we examined emotional eating rather than binge eating and thus, it remains unclear whether our results generalize to full-blown binge eating. Although emotional eating scores are highly correlated with traditional measures of binge eating,30, 41 it will be important to replicate our findings for binge eating episodes.

Second, although we view our use of a community sample of women as a significant strength that simultaneously increased samples sizes/power as well as generalizability to the population at large, it limited our ability to examine moderating associations in women with diagnosable eating disorders. Our use of a community sample also likely resulted in smaller effect sizes for hormones and dietary restraint, as stronger associations between ovarian hormones and dysregulated eating have been found in studies of women with BN1, 4 and those with higher levels of emotional eating.3 Additional research investigating moderators of hormone/emotional eating associations in clinical samples is needed to replicate our findings and test hypotheses of differential effects of restraint on hormone/behavior associations in more disturbed groups. The need for these studies is underscored by the fact that the dietary restraint × estradiol interaction was only of trend-level significance in our community sample. Future clinical studies should assess a large number of participants and directly measure daily food intake to determine effect sizes (i.e., trend-level or significant effects?) and provide objective measures of dietary restraint and binge eating episodes.

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIMH.

  • a

    To further ensure that our use of twins did not unduly influence results, we conducted analyses in samples of MZ and DZ twins only. The pattern of results was identical to that in the full sample (data not shown).

  • b

    Because BMI and dietary restraint were correlated (r =. 23), we also ran the model for BMI with restraint as a covariate, and the model for restraint with BMI as a covariate. Results were identical to those presented herein, with significant estradiol × progesterone interactions but no significant two- or three-way interactions between the moderators and hormones.

References

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Introduction
  4. Method
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. References