Accidental awareness during intended general anaesthesia with postoperative explicit recall (AWR) can be a devastating experience for patients, frequently leading to posttraumatic stress disorder [1]. In studies conducted outside of the UK, AWR has been estimated to affect 1 to 2 per thousand surgical patients [2-4]. If this incidence is extrapolated to the UK, it would suggest that thousands of surgical patients a year could be experiencing this complication. As such, AWR is an important adverse event for patients that requires the attention of healthcare practitioners and policy makers. In a joint publication in British Journal of Anaesthesia and Anaesthesia [5, 6], Pandit and colleagues report the results of the first phase of the 5th National Audit Project (NAP5) of the Royal College of Anaesthetists in collaboration with the Association of Anaesthetists of Great Britain and Ireland. The findings of the first phase of NAP5 prompt a reconsideration of the detection and incidence of AWR.

The difficulty in detecting awareness during an intended general anaesthetic is manifest. Conscious experience is inherently subjective, the neurobiology of consciousness has yet to be elucidated, and as such there is currently no gold standard test for identifying awareness. Despite the limitations, studies that have sought to detect awareness with evoked responsiveness as the outcome measure (e.g. asking a patient at regular intervals to squeeze an object and ensuring that the hand holding the object is not pharmacologically paralysed) have found that about a third of patients experience episodes of awareness during intended general anaesthesia [7]. Fortunately, there is a low probability that these episodes result in explicit memory formation. Whether awareness without explicit recall has any clinical relevance is a matter of debate. It is therefore appropriate that NAP5 focused on AWR, a complication that clearly precipitates patient distress and has demonstrable clinical sequelae.

The National Audit Projects seek to shed light on issues that are important to patients and anaesthetists alike. The overarching aim of NAP5 is to ascertain, over one full year (1st June 2012 to 31st May 2013), how many patients in the UK (and The Republic of Ireland, where the audit will be running in parallel) spontaneously report AWR, and to elucidate relevant risk factors for AWR ( The first or preliminary phase of the NAP5 endeavour, which is reported by Pandit and colleagues [5, 6], is a survey in which anaesthetists around the UK were asked about their practices regarding the prevention of AWR and their recollections of any AWR reports that were brought to their attention during 2011. This phase 1 NAP5 survey is impressive in its scope and accomplishments; 100% of contacted centres (329 hospitals) participated, and a remarkable 82% of senior anaesthetists (7125) responded to the survey. Based on cases of awareness that became known to anaesthetists during 2011, the survey suggests that the current incidence of AWR in the UK is about 1 in 15 000. This is much lower than the 1 to 2 per 1000 incidence that has been found in several prospective studies that have assessed AWR [2-4].

This is not the first time the field of anaesthesia has been confronted with an investigation reporting an incidence of AWR one order of magnitude lower than that found in prospective studies. In 2007, Pollard and colleagues described a study of AWR in a regional medical system in the US and found an incidence of approximately 1 in 15 000, which is commensurate with the findings of the NAP5 survey [8]. This dramatically lower incidence was attributed either to differences in anaesthetic technique or to the method of assessing awareness. It will be beneficial to consider the results of the NAP5 survey in light of these potential explanations. First, the incidence of AWR could be much lower in the UK than has been reported previously in other countries (i.e. ‘under the rate’). Second, the NAP5 survey substantially underestimated the incidence of AWR (i.e. ‘under the radar’). Third, both explanations may be partially correct. It is unclear, based on the design of NAP5, whether we can adjudicate definitively among these options.

If the incidence of AWR is indeed lower in the UK than previously reported elsewhere, it is important to consider why this might be in order to reinforce good anaesthetic techniques, and to inform practitioners in other countries. The first and most compelling explanation could simply be that, unlike previous studies, the NAP5 survey is both national and comprehensive. As such, all general anaesthetics in all settings are potentially captured, of which most are probably low-risk outpatient surgical procedures. A second reason might be that the laryngeal mask airway and other supraglottic devices have commonly been used during general anaesthesia in the UK for many years. When a supraglottic device is used, neuromuscular blocking drugs are typically not administered, and this would probably mitigate both the incidence and the severity of AWR 2, 9]. Surprisingly, however, Sandin and colleagues found an AWR incidence of 1 in 1000 even when neuromuscular blocking drugs were not administered [2]. A third factor might be that the favourable staffing ratio and extensive training of anaesthetists in the UK are associated with increased vigilance and decreased AWR. A fourth possibility is that practitioners have changed clinical behavior following studies suggesting that AWR can be decreased with protocols incorporating electroencephalogram (EEG)-based monitors or end-tidal anesthetic concentration (ETAC) alerts [10-12]. The low utilization of EEG-based monitors (only 1.8% routine use) among participant anaesthetists partially argues against the EEG approach, but practitioners might have set alerts based on low ETAC thresholds. A fifth contributor might be an avoidance of total intravenous anaesthesia (TIVA); the risk of AWR is higher with TIVA than with volatile-based anaesthesia [13-15]. However, it is likely that TIVA is more popular in the UK than in the US. Therefore, low utilisation of TIVA probably does not explain the low practitioner-reported incidence of AWR found in the NAP5 survey, although the widespread use of target-controlled infusion pumps in the UK might increase the safety of TIVA (in relation to propofol under-dosing).

In contrast to the hypothesised reasons for a uniquely low incidence of AWR in the UK, past studies suggest that phase 1 of the NAP5 project substantially under-detected the true incidence of AWR in the UK. It is important to consider possible explanations for this discrepancy in order to assist interpretation of phase 2 of the NAP5 project, which is designed to determine prospectively how many patients report AWR in the UK. The most important limitation of NAP5 phase 1 is that AWR was detected through intermediates (i.e. senior anaesthetists) based on patient self-reports rather than through direct patient interviews. It has been demonstrated that patients are unlikely to report spontaneously an AWR event to an anaesthetist [16], perhaps because of the brief interaction and limited relationship. Based on data from a single hospital complex in the US, a retrospective audit of spontaneous patient reports revealed a six-fold lower incidence of AWR [17] than a study in which patients were questioned directly [4]. Any study that relies on spontaneous patient reports of AWR rather than proactive questioning of all patients is likely to underestimate considerably the incidence of AWR. Furthermore, in the phase 1 NAP5 practitioner survey, as patients were not questioned directly, it is self-evident that patients were not asked specifically if they remembered anything during the period of intended general anaesthetic, which is one of the questions in the modified Brice interview [18]. Based on discrepant findings between studies that have asked this question [2, 3] and those that haven't [8], this targeted query appears to be important. This argument is made even more compelling by a study that compared the detected incidence of awareness in a single patient population through two different methods: routine postoperative interviews vs structured questionnaires (i.e. modified Brice interviews) that explicitly asked patients whether they remembered anything during the period in which they were intended to be anaesthetised [19]. In this study, the detected incidence was five times higher when the explicit question was asked [19]. As such, any study that does not ask patients explicitly about AWR is likely to underestimate considerably the incidence of AWR. Lastly, studies suggest that patients might not remember or might not choose to report that they were aware immediately after their surgery [2, 3, 12, 16]. One notable case report described a patient who chose not to disclose an AWR event during two structured and explicit interviews within a month of surgery, but mentioned it spontaneously during a one-year follow-up interview not related to detection of AWR [20]. Patients should be interviewed for AWR at least one to four weeks after their surgery [2, 3, 12], recognising that even this approach will probably not capture all AWR experiences.

In addition to the interesting findings on AWR reports, the phase 1 NAP5 survey found a low use of EEG-based monitors and a general lack of policies to prevent and manage AWR [5]. It is therefore appropriate and encouraging that organisations representing anaesthetists in the UK as well as the National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE) are currently in the process of addressing this policy vacuum. Various quality improvement measures or protocols are under consideration for effectiveness and efficiency, in addition to measures that have previously been recommended, such as the avoidance or restricted administration of neuromuscular blocking drugs [9, 21, 22]. Evidence has accumulated over the past few years that might be helpful in framing clinical decision pathways [23]. Practitioners should be encouraged to set alerts for a threshold low ETAC when using a volatile-based anaesthetic, as this practice probably decreases AWR [4, 12, 24]. If neuromuscular blocking drugs are deemed necessary, deliberation should be given to using a volatile-based technique, as TIVA is associated with a higher incidence of AWR [12-14]. If TIVA is indicated or chosen (and neuromuscular blocking drugs are administered), a proprietary or non-proprietary EEG-based monitor should be used in order to decrease the likelihood of AWR [11, 15]. The recent NICE guidelines, issued in November 2012, endorse the recommendation that an EEG-based monitor should be considered for patients receiving TIVA. In addition, the NICE guidelines recommend that such monitors should be considered for patients: i) assessed to be at higher risk of AWR; ii) who are liable to experience haemodynamic instability at typical anaesthetic doses; or iii) for whom there is a hypothetical concern, owing to their perceived vulnerability, that typical anaesthetic doses could be injurious ( It is worth noting that a non-proprietary EEG can be used inexpensively. Information from the EEG and from non-proprietary processed EEG indices might yield comparable information to commercially available devices [25-28].

Policy makers and practitioners should not curtail their systematic efforts to eradicate AWR based on the first-phase NAP5 results, which could be interpreted as pointing to the rarity of AWR in the UK. As noted by Pandit and colleagues, the methodology of the NAP5 survey renders it vulnerable to under-detection of AWR [5, 6]. Although both phases of NAP5 might ultimately provide substantial underestimates of the true incidence of AWR in the UK owing to their reliance on spontaneous patient reports, we nonetheless eagerly anticipate the next phase of NAP5, which will provide a rich source of valuable data as the first national and comprehensive prospective assessment of AWR.

Competing interests

  1. Top of page
  2. Competing interests
  3. References

No external funding and no competing interests declared.

This article is published simultaneously in Anaesthesia and the British Journal of Anaesthesia.


  1. Top of page
  2. Competing interests
  3. References
  • 1
    Leslie K, Chan MT, Myles PS, Forbes A, McCulloch TJ. Posttraumatic stress disorder in aware patients from the B-aware trial. Anesthesia and Analgesia 2010; 110: 8238.
  • 2
    Sandin RH, Enlund G, Samuelsson P, Lennmarken C. Awareness during anaesthesia: a prospective case study. Lancet 2000; 355: 70711.
  • 3
    Sebel PS, Bowdle TA, Ghoneim MM, et al. The incidence of awareness during anesthesia: a multicenter United States study. Anesthesia and Analgesia 2004; 99: 8339.
  • 4
    Mashour GA, Shanks A, Tremper KK, et al. Prevention of intraoperative awareness with explicit recall in an unselected surgical population: a randomized comparative effectiveness trial. Anesthesiology 2012; 117: 71725.
  • 5
    Pandit JJ, Cook TM, Jonker WR, O'Sullivan E. A national survey of anaesthetists (NAP5 baseline) to estimate an annual incidence of accidental awareness during general anaesthesia in the UK. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2013; 110: 5019.
  • 6
    Pandit JJ, Cook TM, Jonker WR, O'Sullivan E. A national survey of anaesthetists (NAP5 baseline) to estimate an annual incidence of accidental awareness during general anaesthesia in the UK. Anaesthesia 2013; 68; 34353.
  • 7
    Sanders RD, Tononi G, Laureys S, Sleigh JW. Unresponsiveness ≠ unconsciousness. Anesthesiology 2012; 116: 94659.
  • 8
    Pollard RJ, Coyle JP, Gilbert RL, Beck JE. Intraoperative awareness in a regional medical system: a review of 3 years’ data. Anesthesiology 2007; 106: 26974.
  • 9
    Ghoneim MM, Block RI, Haffarnan M, Mathews MJ. Awareness during anesthesia: risk factors, causes and sequelae: a review of reported cases in the literature. Anesthesia and Analgesia 2009; 108: 52735.
  • 10
    Ekman A, Lindholm ML, Lennmarken C, Sandin R. Reduction in the incidence of awareness using BIS monitoring. Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 2004; 48: 206.
  • 11
    Myles PS, Leslie K, McNeil J, Forbes A, Chan MT. Bispectral index monitoring to prevent awareness during anaesthesia: the B-Aware randomised controlled trial. Lancet 2004; 363: 175763.
  • 12
    Avidan MS, Zhang L, Burnside BA, et al. Anesthesia awareness and the bispectral index. New England Journal of Medicine 2008; 358: 1097108.
  • 13
    Errando CL, Sigl JC, Robles M, et al. Awareness with recall during general anaesthesia: a prospective observational evaluation of 4001 patients. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2008; 101: 17885.
  • 14
    Xu L, Wu AS, Yue Y. The incidence of intra-operative awareness during general anesthesia in China: a multi-center observational study. Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 2009; 53: 87382.
  • 15
    Zhang C, Xu L, Ma YQ, et al. Bispectral index monitoring prevent awareness during total intravenous anesthesia: a prospective, randomized, double-blinded, multi-center controlled trial. Chinese Medical Journal 2011; 124: 36649.
  • 16
    Moerman N, Bonke B, Oosting J. Awareness and recall during general anesthesia. Facts and feelings. Anesthesiology 1993; 79: 45464.
  • 17
    Mashour GA, Wang LY, Turner CR, Vandervest JC, Shanks A, Tremper KK. A retrospective study of intraoperative awareness with methodological implications. Anesthesia and Analgesia 2009; 108: 5216.
  • 18
    Brice DD, Hetherington RR, Utting JE. A simple study of awareness and dreaming during anaesthesia. British Journal of Anaesthesia 1970; 42: 53542.
  • 19
    Mashour GA, Kent C, Picton P, et al. Assessment of intraoperative awareness with explicit recall: a comparison of two methods. Anesthesia and Analgesia 2013; (in press).
  • 20
    Villafranca AJ, Arenson BG, Avidan MS, Glick D, Mashour GA, Jacobsohn E. Volitional delay of self-reported outcomes: insights from a case of intraoperative awareness with explicit recall. Anesthesia and Analgesia 2013; 116: 3657.
  • 21
    Mashour GA, Orser BA, Avidan MS. Intraoperative awareness: from neurobiology to clinical practice. Anesthesiology 2011; 114: 121833.
  • 22
    Practice advisory for intraoperative awareness and brain function monitoring. a report by the American Society Of Anesthesiologists Task Force on Intraoperative Awareness. Anesthesiology 2006; 104: 84764.
  • 23
    Avidan MS, Mashour GA. Prevention of intraoperative awareness with explicit recall: making sense of the evidence. Anesthesiology 2013; 118: 44956.
  • 24
    Avidan MS, Jacobsohn E, Glick D, et al. Prevention of intraoperative awareness in a high-risk surgical population. New England Journal of Medicine 2011; 365: 591600.
  • 25
    Barnard JP, Bennett C, Voss LJ, Sleigh JW. Can anaesthetists be taught to interpret the effects of general anaesthesia on the electroencephalogram? Comparison of performance with the BIS and spectral entropy. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2007; 99: 5327.
  • 26
    Bennett C, Voss LJ, Barnard JP, Sleigh JW. Practical use of the raw electroencephalogram waveform during general anesthesia: the art and science. Anesthesia and Analgesia 2009; 109: 53950.
  • 27
    Bottros MM, Palanca BJ, Mashour GA, et al. Estimation of the bispectral index by anesthesiologists: an inverse turing test. Anesthesiology 2011; 114: 1093101.
  • 28
    Olofsen E, Sleigh JW, Dahan A. Permutation entropy of the electroencephalogram: a measure of anaesthetic drug effect. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2008; 101: 81021.