SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Acredolo, L. P., Adams, A., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1984). The role of self-produced movement and visual tracking in infant spatial orientation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 312327.
  • Bahrick, L. E., Lickliter, R., & Flom, R. (2004). Intersensory redundancy guides the development of selective attention, perception and cognition in infancy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 99102.
    Direct Link:
  • Bai, D. L., & Bertenthal, B. I. (1992). Locomotor status and the development of spatial search skills. Child Development, 63, 215226.
  • Benson, J. B., & Uzgiris, I. C. (1985). Effect of self-initiated locomotion on infant search activity. Developmental Psychology, 21, 923931.
  • Bertenthal, B. I. (1996). Origins and early development of perception, action, and representation. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 431459.
  • Berthier, N. E., DeBlois, S., Poirier, C. R., Novak, M. A., & Clifton, R. K. (2000). Where's the ball? Two- and three-year-olds reason about unseen events. Developmental Psychology, 36, 394401.
  • Bruner, J. S., Olver, R. R., & Greenfield, P. M. (1966). Studies in cognitive growth. New York: Wiley.
  • Bülthoff, H., & Edelman, S. (1992). Psychophysical support for a two-dimensional view interpolation theory of object recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 89, 6064.
  • Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149219.
  • Csibra, G., Biró, S., Koós, O., & Gergely, G. (2003). One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively. Cognitive Science, 27, 111133.
  • Ehrlich, S. B., Levine, S. C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The importance of gesture in children's spatial reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 42, 12591268.
  • Fenson, L., Kagan, J., Kearsley, R. B., & Zelazo, P. R. (1976). The developmental progression of manipulative play in the first two years. Child Development, 47, 232236.
  • Fogassi, L., & Luppino, G. (2005). Motor functions of the parietal lobe. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 626631.
  • Frick, A., Daum, M. M., Walser, S., & Mast, F. W. (2009). Motor processes in children's mental rotation. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10, 1840.
  • Frick, A., Daum, M. M., Wilson, M., & Wilkening, F. (2009). Effects of action on children's and adults' mental imagery. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 3451.
  • Gibson, E. J. (1988). Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 141.
  • Gibson, E. J., & Pick, A. D. (2000). An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Hespos, S. J., & Baillargeon, R. (2006). Décalage in infants' knowledge about occlusion and containment events: Converging evidence from action tasks. Cognition, 99, B31B41.
  • Hespos, S. J., & Rochat, P. (1997). Dynamic mental representation in infancy. Cognition, 64, 153188.
  • Hood, B., Carey, S., & Prasada, S. (2000). Predicting the outcomes of physical events: Two-year-olds fail to reveal knowledge of solidity and support. Child Development, 71, 15401554.
  • Huttenlocher, J., & Presson, C. C. (1973). Mental rotation and the perspective problem. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 277299.
  • Jäncke, L., & Jordan, K. (2007). Functional neuroanatomy of mental rotation performance. In F. Mast & L. Jancke (Eds.), Spatial processing in navigation, imagery and perception (pp. 183207). New York: Springer.
  • Keen, R. (2003). Representation of objects and events: Why do infants look so smart and toddlers look so dumb? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 7983.
    Direct Link:
  • Keen, R., & Berthier, N. E. (2004). Continuities and discontinuities in infants' representation of objects and events. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 243279). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.
  • Kellman, P. J., & Arterberry, M. E. (1998). The cradle of knowledge: Development of perception in infancy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Lockman, J. J. (2000). A perception–action perspective on tool use development. Child Development, 71, 137144.
  • Loucks, J., & Sommerville, J. A. (2012). The role of motor experience in understanding action function: The case of the precision grasp. Child Development, 83, 801809.
  • Mast, F., Bamert, L., & Newby, N. (2007). Mind over matter? Imagined body movements and their neuronal correlates. In F. Mast & L. Jancke (Eds.), Spatial processing in navigation, imagery and perception (pp. 353368). New York: Springer.
  • McCarty, M. E., Clifton, R. K., & Collard, R. R. (1999). Problem solving in infancy: The emergence of an action plan. Developmental Psychology, 35, 10911101.
  • McCarty, M. E., Clifton, R. K., & Collard, R. R. (2001). The beginnings of tool use by infants and toddlers. Infancy, 2, 233256.
  • Meltzoff, A. M. (1988). Infant imitation and memory: Nine-month-olds in immediate and deferred tests. Child Development, 59, 217225.
  • Möhring, W., & Frick, A. (2013). Touching up mental rotation: Effects of manual experience on 6-month-old infants' mental object rotation. Child Development. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/cdev.12065.
  • Moore, D. S., & Johnson, S. P. (2008). Mental rotation in human infants: A sex difference. Psychological Science, 19, 10631066.
    Direct Link:
  • Moore, D. S., & Johnson, S. P. (2011). Mental rotation of dynamic three-dimensional stimuli by 3-month-old infants. Infancy, 16, 435445.
  • Nardini, M., Burgess, N., Breckenridge, K., & Atkinson, J. (2006). Differential developmental trajectories for egocentric, environmental and intrinsic frames of reference in spatial memory. Cognition, 101, 153172.
  • Needham, A. (2000). Improvements in object exploration skills may facilitate the development of object segregation in early infancy. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 131156.
  • Needham, A., Barrett, T., & Peterman, K. (2002). A pick me up for infants' exploratory skills: Early simulated experiences reaching for objects using “sticky” mittens enhances young infants' object exploration skills. Infant Behavior & Development, 25, 279295.
  • Newcombe, N. S., & Frick, A. (2010). Early education for spatial intelligence: Why, what, and how. Mind, Brain, and Education, 4, 102111.
  • Noller, K., & Ingrisano, D. (1984). Cross-sectional study of gross and fine motor development: Birth to 6 years of age. Physical Therapy, 64, 308316.
  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: International Universities Press. (Original work published 1936)
  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child's conception of space (F. J. Langdon & J. L. Lunzer, Trans.). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1948)
  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1971). Mental imagery in the child: A study of the development of imaginal representation (P. A. Chilton, Trans.). New York: Basic. (Original work published 1966)
  • Pick, H. L., & Lockman, J. J. (1981). From frames of reference to spatial representations. In L. S. Liben, A. H. Patterson, & N. S. Newcombe (Eds.), Spatial representation and behavior across the life span (pp. 3961). New York: Academic Press.
  • Quinn, P. C., & Liben, L. S. (2008). A sex difference in mental rotation in young infants. Psychological Science, 19, 10671070.
    Direct Link:
  • Rochat, P., & Hespos, S. J. (1996). Tracking and anticipation of invisible spatial transformation by 4- to 8-month-old infants. Cognitive Development, 11, 317.
  • Shimada, S., & Hiraki, K. (2006). Infant's brain responses to live and televised action. Neuroimage, 32, 930939.
  • Simons, D. J., & Wang, R. F. (1998). Perceiving real-world viewpoint changes. Psychological Science, 9, 315320.
    Direct Link:
  • Sommerville, J. A., Hildebrand, E. A., & Crane, C. C. (2008). Experience matters: The impact of doing versus watching on infants' subsequent perception of tool use events. Developmental Psychology, 44, 12491256.
  • Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions. Cognition, 96, B1B11.
  • Soska, K. C., Adolph, K. E., & Johnson, S. P. (2010). Systems in development: Motor skill acquisition facilitates three-dimensional object completion. Developmental Psychology, 46, 129138.
  • Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., & Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605632.
  • Wang, S., Baillargeon, R., & Paterson, S. (2005). Detecting continuity violations in infancy: A new account and new evidence from covering and tube events. Cognition, 95, 129173.
  • Wang, S., & Kohne, L. (2007). Visual experience enhances infants' use of task-relevant information in an action task. Developmental Psychology, 43, 15131522.
  • Wexler, M., Kosslyn, S. M., & Berthoz, A. (1998). Motor processes in mental rotation. Cognition, 68, 7794.
  • Wilcox, T., Woods, R., Chapa, C., & McCurry, S. (2007). Multisensory exploration and object individuation in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 43, 479495.
  • Willatts, P. (1997). Beyond the “couch potato” infant: How infants use their knowledge to regulate action, solve problems, and achieve goals. In G. Bremner, A. Slater, & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Infant development: Recent advances (pp. 109135). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • Willatts, P. (1999). Development of means–end behavior in young infants: Pulling a support to retrieve a distant object. Developmental Psychology, 35, 651667.
  • Wohlschläger, A., & Wohlschläger, A. (1998). Mental and manual rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 397412.
  • Wolpert, D. M., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Motor prediction. Current Biology, 11, R729R732.