SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Avraamides, M. N. (2003). Spatial updating of environments described in texts. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 402431.
  • Avraamides, M. N., Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., & Golledge, R. G. (2004). Functional equivalence of spatial representations derived from vision and language: Evidence from allocentric judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 801814.
  • Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, W. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. In M. de Vega, A. M. Glenberg & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols, embodiment, and meaning (pp. 245283). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Borg, I., & Groenen, P. J. F. (2005). Modern multidimensional scaling: Theory and applications. New York: Springer.
  • Bryant, D. J., Lanca, M., & Tversky, B. (1995). Spatial concepts and perception of physical and diagrammed scenes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81, 531546.
  • Bryant, D. J., & Tversky, B. (1999). Mental representations of spatial relations from diagrams and models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 25, 137156.
  • Coventry, K. R., & Garrod, S. C. (2004). Saying, seeing and acting: The psychological semantics of spatial prepositions. New York: Psychology Press.
  • De Vega, M., Glenberg, A. M., & Graesser, A. C. (Eds.) (2008). Symbols and embodiment: Debates on meaning and cognition. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • De Vega, M., & Rodrigo, M. J. (2001). Updating mental frameworks mediated by pointing and labeling under physical and imaginary rotation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 369393.
  • Downs, R. M., & Stea, D. (1977). Maps in minds: Reflections on cognitive mapping. New York: Harper and Row.
  • Dryden, I. L., & Mardia, K. V. (1988). Statistical shape analysis. Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Ehrich, V., & Koster, C. (1983). Discourse organization and sentence form: The structure of room descriptions in Dutch. Discourse Processes, 6, 169195.
  • Ehrlich, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1982). Spatial descriptions and referential continuity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 296306.
  • Firth, J. R. (1957). Papers in linguistics 1934–1951. London: Oxford University Press.
  • Fonstad, K. W. (2001). The atlas of Middle-Earth. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
  • Franklin, N., & Tversky, B. (1990). Searching imagined environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 6376.
  • Freundschuh, S. M., & Mercer, D. (1995). Spatial cognitive representations of story worlds acquired from maps and narrative. Geographical Systems, 2, 217233.
  • Friedman, A., & Kohler, B. (2003). Bidimensional regression: A method for assessing the configural similarity of cognitive maps and other two-dimensional data. Psychological Methods, 8, 468491.
  • Friedman, A., & Montello, D. R. (2006). Global-scale location and distance estimates: Common representations and strategies in absolute and relative judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 333346.
  • Landauer, T., McNamara, D. S., Dennis, S., & Kintsch, W. (Eds.) (2007). Handbook of latent semantic analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Loomis, J. M., Lippa, Y., Klatzky, R. L., & Golledge, R. G. (2002). Spatial updating of locations specified by 3-D sound and spatial language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 335345.
  • Louwerse, M. M. (2008). Embodied representations are encoded in language. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15, 838844.
  • Louwerse, M. M. (2011a). Stormy seas and cloudy skies: Conceptual processing is (still) linguistic and perceptual. Frontiers in Psychology: Cognition, 2, 14.
  • Louwerse, M. M. (2011b). Symbol interdependency in symbolic and embodied cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3, 273302.
  • Louwerse, M. M., Cai, Z., Hu, X., Ventura, M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2006). Cognitively inspired natural-language based knowledge representations: Further explorations of latent semantic analysis. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence Tools, 15, 10211039.
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Connell, L. (2011). A taste of words: Linguistic context and perceptual simulation predict the modality of words. Cognitive Science, 35, 381398.
  • Louwerse, M. M., Hutchinson, S., & Cai, Z. (2012). The Chinese route argument: Predicting the longitude and latitude of cities in China and the Middle East using statistical linguistic frequencies. Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 695–700). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2008). Language comprehension is both embodied and symbolic. In M. de Vega, A. Glenberg, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols and embodiment: Debates on meaning and cognition (pp. 309326). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2010). The linguistic and embodied nature of conceptual processing. Cognition, 114, 96104.
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Zwaan, R. A. (2009). Language encodes geographical information. Cognitive Science, 33, 5173.
  • Malt, B., & Wolff, P. (2010). Words and the mind: How words capture human experience. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Montello, D. R., & Freundschuh, S. M. (1995). Sources of spatial knowledge and their implications for GIS: An introduction. Geographical Systems, 2, 169176.
  • Morrow, D. G., Greenspan, S. L., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Accessibility and situation models in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 165187.
  • Paivio, A. (1978). Imagery, language, and semantic memory. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 5, 3147.
  • Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Radvansky, G. A., Copeland, D. E., & Zwaan, R. A. (2005). A novel study: Investigating the structure of narrative and autobiographical memories. Memory, 13, 796814.
  • Richardson, A. E., Montello, D., & Hegarty, M. (1999). Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps, and from navigation in real and virtual environments. Memory and Cognition, 27, 741750.
  • Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2001). Imagery and text: A dual coding theory of reading and writing. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Schönemann, P. H., & Caroll, R. M. (1970). Fitting one matrix to another under choice of a central dilation and a rigid motion. Psychometrika, 35, 245255.
  • Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1992a). Descriptions and depictions of environments. Memory and Cognition, 20, 483496.
  • Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1992b). Spatial mental models derived from survey and route descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 261282.
  • Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1996). Perspective in spatial descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 371391.
  • Thorndyke, P. W., & Hayes-Roth, B. (1982). Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 560589.
  • Tobler, W. (1994). Bidimensional regression. Geographical Analysis, 26, 186212.
  • Tolkien, J. R. R. (20021937). The hobbit. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
  • Tolkien, J. R. R. (20021954). The lord of the rings. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
  • Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189208.
  • Tversky, B., Lee, P. U., & Mainwaring, S. (1999). Why speakers mix perspectives. Journal of Spatial Cognition and Computation, 1, 399412.
  • Van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York: Academic Press.
  • Waller, D., & Haun, D. B. M. (2003). Scaling techniques for modeling directional knowledge. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 285293.
  • Young, F.W., & Harris, D.F. (1990). Multidimensional scaling: Procedure ALSCAL. In J.J. Norusis (Ed.), SPSS base system user’s guide (pp. 396461). Chicago, IL: SPSS Inc.