SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Amster, H. (1964). Semantic satiation and generation: Learning? Adaptation? Psychological Bulletin, 62, 273286.
  • Anderson, J. R. (1974). Retrieval of prepositional information from long-term memory. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 451474.
  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 261295.
  • Balota, D. A., & Lorch, R. F. (1986). Depth of automatic spreading activation: Mediated priming effects in pronunciation but not in lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 336345.
  • Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., & Watson, J. M. (2008). Beyond mean response latency: Response time distributional analyses of semantic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 495523.
  • Bibitchkov, D., Herrmann, J. M., & Geisel, T. (2002). Pattern storage and processing in attractor networks with short-time synaptic dynamics. Network: Computation in Neural Systems, 13, 115129.
  • Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115147.
  • Borowsky, R., & Besner, D. (1993). Visual word recognition: A multistage activation model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 813840.
  • Bowers, J. S. (2009). On the biological plausibility of grandmother cells: Implications for neural network theories in psychology and neuroscience. Psychological Review, 116, 220251.
  • Brunel, N., & Lavigne, F. (2009). Semantic priming in a cortical network model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 23002319.
  • Büchel, C., & Friston, K. J. (1997). Modulation of connectivity in visual pathways by attention: Cortical interactions evaluated with structural equation modelling and fMRI. Cerebral Cortex, 7, 768778.
  • Cohen, J. D., & Servan-Schreiber, D. (1992). Context, cortex, and dopamine: A connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychological Review, 99, 4577.
  • Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407428.
  • Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240247.
  • Coull, J. T. (1998). Neural correlates of attention and arousal: Insights from electrophysiological, functional neuroimaging and psychopharmacology. Progress in Neurobiology, 55, 343361.
  • Dalrymple-Alford, E. E., & Marmurek, H. H. C. (1999). Semantic priming in fully recurrent network models of lexical knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 758775.
  • Deese, J. (1962). On the structure of associative meaning. Psychological Review, 69, 161175.
  • Flekkoy, K. (1973). Associative frequency and response latency. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 24, 199202.
  • Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT press.
  • Franklin, M. S., Dien, J., Neely, J. H., Waterson, L. D., & Huber, L. (2007). Semantic priming modulates the N400, N300, and N400RP. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 10531068.
  • Gazzaniga, M. S. (2000). Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: Does the corpus callosum enable the human condition?. Brain, 123, 12931326.
  • Goldstein, M. J. (1961). The relationship between anxiety and oral word association performance. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62, 468471.
  • de Groot, A. M. B. (1984). Primed lexical decision: Combined effects of the proportion of related prime-target pairs and the stimulus-onset asynchrony of prime and target. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 253280.
  • de Groot, A. M. B. (1985). Word-context effects in word naming and lexical decision. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 281297.
  • de Groot, A. M. B., Thomassen, A. J., & Hudson, P. T. (1986). Primed lexical decision: The effect of varying the stimulus-onset asynchrony of prime and target. Acta Psychologica, 61, 1736.
  • Harding, A., Halliday, G., & Kril, J. (1998). Variation in hippocampal neuron number with age and brain volume. Cerebral Cortex, 8, 710718.
  • Herrmann, M., Ruppin, E., & Usher, M. (1993). A neural model of the dynamic activation of memory. Biological Cybernetics, 68, 455463.
  • Hopfield, J. J. (1982). Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 79, 25542558.
  • Hopfield, J. J. (1984). Neurons with graded response have collective computational properties like those of two-state neurons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 81, 30883092.
  • Horn, D., & Usher, M. (1989). Neural networks with dynamical thresholds. Physical Review A, 40, 10361040.
  • Huber, D. E.. (2008). Causality in time: Explaining away the future and the past. In M. Oaksford & N. Chater (Eds.), The probabilistic mind: Prospects for rational models of cognition (pp. 351376). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Huber, D. E., & O’Reilly, R. C. (2003). Persistence and accommodation in short-term priming and other perceptual paradigms: Temporal segregation through synaptic depression. Cognitive Science, 27, 403430.
  • Hutchison, K. A. (2003). Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 785813.
  • Jones, L. L. (2012). Prospective and retrospective processing in associative mediated priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 5267.
  • Jones, M. N., Kintsch, W., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (2006). High-dimensional semantic space accounts of priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 534552.
  • Kahan, T. A., Neely, J. H., & Forsythe, W. J. (1999). Dissociated backward priming effects in lexical decision and pronunciation tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 105110.
  • Kawamoto, A. H., & Anderson, J. A. (1985). A neural network model of multistable perception. Acta Psychologica, 59, 3565.
  • Knill, D. C., & Pouget, A. (2004). The Bayesian brain: The role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends in Neurosciences, 27, 712719.
  • Kropff, E., & Treves, A. (2007). The complexity of latching transitions in large scale cortical networks. Natural Computing, 6, 169185.
  • Lambert, W. E., & Jakobovits, L. A. (1960). Verbal satiation and changes in the intensity of meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 376383.
  • Lavigne, F., & Darmon, N. (2008). Dopaminergic neuromodulation of semantic priming in a cortical network model. Neuropsychologia, 46, 30743087.
  • Lerner, I., Bentin, S., & Shriki, O. (2012). Excessive attractor instability accounts for semantic priming in schizophrenia. PLoS ONE 7(7): e40663. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040663
  • Lorch, R. F. (1982). Priming and search processes in semantic memory: A test of three models of spreading activation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 468492.
  • Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without association: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 618630.
  • Lupker, S. J. (1984). Semantic priming without association: A second look. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 23, 709733.
  • Mandler, G. (2005). The consciousness continuum: From ‘qualia’ to ‘free will’. Psychological Research, 69, 330337.
  • Masson, M. E. J. (1995). A distributed memory model of semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 323.
  • Mato, G. (1999). Stochastic resonance using noise generated by a neural network. Physical Review E, 59, 33393343.
  • McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375407.
  • McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1992). Spreading activation versus compound cue accounts of priming: Mediated priming revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 11551172.
  • McNamara, T. P.. (2005). Semantic priming: Perspectives from memory and word recognition. New York: Psychology Press.
  • McNamara, T. P., & Holbrook, J. B.. (2003). Semantic memory and priming. In A. F. Healy & R. W. Proctor (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Experimental psychology (pp. 447474). New York: Wiley.
  • McRae, K., de Sa, V., & Seidenberg, M. S.. (1993). Modeling property interactions in accessing conceptual memory. In J. W. Kintsch (Ed.), Proceedings of the 15th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 729734). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227234.
  • Meyn, S. P., & Tweedie, R. L.. (1993). Markov chains and stochastic stability. New York: Springer-Verlag.
  • Moss, H. E., Hare, M. L., Day, P., & Tyler, L. K. (1994). A distributed memory model of the associative boost in semantic priming. Connection Science, 6, 413427.
  • Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 22254.
  • Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading (pp. 264336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A.. (1998). The University of South Florida word association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. http://www.usf.edu/FreeAssociation/. Accessed October 14, 2010.
  • Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (2004). The University of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 402407.
  • Peterson, R. R., & Simpson, G. B. (1989). Effect of backward priming on word recognition in single-word and sentence contexts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 10201032.
  • Pfister, J. P., Dayan, P., & Lengyel, M. (2010). Synapses with short-term plasticity are optimal estimators of presynaptic membrane potentials. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 12711275.
  • Plaut, D. C. (1995). Semantic and associative priming in a distributed attractor network. In J. F. Lehman & J. D. Moore (Eds.), Proceedings of the 17th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3742). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Plaut, D. C. (1997). Structure and function in the lexical system: Insights from distributed models of naming and lexical decision. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 767808.
  • Plaut, D. C., & Booth, J. R. (2000). Individual and developmental differences in semantic priming: Empirical and computational support for a single-mechanism account of lexical processing. Psychological Review, 107, 786823.
  • Plaut, D. C., & McClelland, J. L. (2010). Locating object knowledge in the brain: A critique of Bowers’ (2009) attempt to revive the grandmother cell hypothesis. Psychological Review, 117, 284288.
  • Quian Quiroga, R., & Kreiman, G. (2010). Measuring sparseness in the brain: Comment on Bowers (2009). Psychological Review, 117, 291299.
  • Quian Quiroga, R., Reddy, R., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., & Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature, 435, 11021107.
  • Ratcliff, R., Gómez, P., & McKoon, G. (2004). A diffusion model account of the lexical decision task. Psychological Review, 111, 142165.
  • Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (1981). Does activation really spread?. Psychological Review, 88, 454462.
  • Rieth, C. A., & Huber, D. E.. (2005). Using a neural network model with synaptic depression to assess the dynamics of feature-based versus configural processing in face identification. In B. G. Bara, L. Barsalou & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 18561861). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.
  • Rolls, E. T., Loh, M., Deco, G., & Winterer, G. (2008). Computational models of schizophrenia and dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 696709.
  • Russo, E., Namboodiri, V. M., Treves, A., & Kropff, E. (2008). Free association transitions in models of cortical latching dynamics. New Journal of Physics, 10, 015008. doi: 10.1088/1367-2630/10/1/015008.
  • Russo, E., Pirmoradian, S., & Treves, A.. (2010). Associative Latching Dynamics vs. Syntax. In R. Wang & F. Gu (Eds.), Advances in Cognitive Neurodynamics (2): Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Cognitive Neurodynamics (pp. 111115). New York: Springer.
  • Schlosberg, H., & Heineman, C. (1950). The relationship between two measures of response strength. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 235247.
  • Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523568.
  • Sharkey, A. J., & Sharkey, N. E. (1992). Weak contextual constraints in text and word priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 543572.
  • Silberman, Y., Miikkulainen, R., & Bentin, S. (2005). Associating unseen events: Semantically mediated formation of episodic associations. Psychological Science, 16, 161166.
    Direct Link:
  • Smith, M. C., Bentin, S., & Spalek, T. M. (2001). Attention constraints of semantic activation during visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 12891298.
  • Spitzer, M. (1997). A cognitive neuroscience view of schizophrenic thought disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23, 2950.
  • Thompson-Schill, S. L., Kurtz, K. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998). Effects of semantic and associative relatedness on automatic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 440458.
  • Tian, X., & Huber, D. E. (2010). Testing an associative account of semantic satiation. Cognitive Psychology, 60, 267290.
  • Treves, A. (2005). Frontal latching networks: A possible neural basis for infinite recursion. Cognitive Neuropsychology22, 276291.
  • Tsodyks, M. V. (1990). Hierarchical associative memory in neural networks with low activity level. Modern Physics Letters B, 4, 259265.
  • Tsodyks, M. V., & Markram, H. (1997). The neural code between neocortical pyramidal neurons depends on neurotransmitter release probability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 94, 719723.
  • Tsodyks, M., Pawelzik, K., & Markram, H. (1998). Neural networks with dynamic synapses. Neural Computation, 10, 821835.
  • Warren, R. M. (1968). Verbal transformation effect and auditory perceptual mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 261270.
  • Waydo, S., Kraskov, A., Quian Quiroga, R., Fried, I., & Koch, C. (2006). Sparse representation in the human medial temporal lobe. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 1023210234.
  • Zador, A. (1998). Impact of synaptic unreliability on the information transmitted by spiking neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79, 12191229.