SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Abrahamson, E., & Rosenkopf, L. (1997). Social network effects on the extent of innovation diffusion: A computer simulation. Organization Science, 8(3), 289309.
  • Akins, C. K., & Zentall, T. R. (1998). Imitation in Japanese quail: The role of reinforcement of the demonstrator's response. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 694697.
  • Anderson, L., & Holt, C. (1997). Information cascades in the laboratory. American Economic Review, 87(5), 847862.
  • Bandura, A. (1965). Vicarious processes: A case of no-trial learning. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology vol. 2 (pp. 155). New York: Academic Press.
  • Banerjee, A. (1992). A simple model of herd behavior. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(3), 797817.
  • Baron, R. S., Vandello, J. A., & Brunsman, B. (1996). The forgotten variable in conformity research: The impact of task performance on social influence. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 71, 915927.
  • Baum, W. M., Richerson, P. J., Efferson, C. M., & Paciotti, B. M. (2004). Cultural evolution in laboratory microsocieties including traditions of rule giving and rule following. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 305326.
  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1995). Why does culture increase human adaptability? Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 125143.
  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2005). The origin and evolution of cultures. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Brown, C. R., & Brown, M. B. (1996). Coloniality in the cliff swallows. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
  • Donchin, E., Giraldeau, L., Valone, T. J., & Wagner, R. H. (2004). Public information: From nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science, 305, 487491.
  • Efferson, C., Lalive, R., Richerson, P. J., McElreath, R., & Lubell, M. (2008). Conformists and mavericks: The empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 5664.
  • Eguíluz, V. M., & Zimmerman, M. G. (2000). Transmission of information and herd behavior: An application to financial markets. Physical Review Letters, 85, 56595662.
  • Galantucci, B. (2009). Experimental semiotics: A new approach for studying communication as a form of joint action. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 393410.
  • Galef, B. G., & Laland, K. N. (2005). Social learning in animals: Empirical studies and theoretical models. BioScience, 55(6), 489499.
  • Gibbons, D. E. (2004). Network structure and innovation ambiguity effects on diffusion in dynamic organizational fields. Academy of Management Journal, 47, 938951.
  • Giraldeau, L., Valone, T. J., & Templeton, J. J. (2002). Potential disadvantages of using socially acquired information. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 357(1427), 15591566.
  • Goldberg, D. E. (1989). Genetic algorithms in search, optimization, and machine learning. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
  • Goldstone, R. L., & Ashpole, B. C. (2004). Human foraging behavior in a virtual environment. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 508514.
  • Goldstone, R. L., Ashpole, B. C., & Roberts, M. E. (2005). Knowledge of resources and competitors in human foraging. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 8187.
  • Goldstone, R. L., & Gureckis, T. M. (2009). Collective behavior. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(3), 412438.
  • Goldstone, R. L., & Janssen, M. A. (2005). Computational models of collective behavior. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 424430.
  • Gureckis, T. M., & Goldstone, R. L. (2009). How you named your child: Understanding the relationship between individual decision making and collective outcomes. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(4), 651674.
  • Healey, P. G. T., Swoboda, N., Umata, I., & King, J. (2007). Graphical language games: Interactional constraints on representational form. Cognitive Science, 31(2), 285309.
  • Hess, C., & Ostrom, E. (Eds.). (2007). Understanding knowledge as a commons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Hurley, S., & Chater, N. (Eds.) (2005). Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Kameda, T., & Nakanishi, D. (2002). Cost–benefit analysis of social/cultural learning in a nonstationary uncertain environment: An evolutionary simulation and an experiment with human subjects. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(5), 373393.
  • Kameda, T., & Nakanishi, D. (2003). Does social/cultural learning increase human adaptability? Rogers' question revisited. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(4), 242260.
  • Kameda, T., & Tindale, R. S. (2006). Groups as adaptive devices: Human docility and group aggregation mechanisms in evolutionary context. In M. Schaller, J. Simpson, & D. T. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolution and social psychology (pp. 317342). New York: Psychology Press.
  • Kameda, T., Tsukasaki, T., Hastie, R., & Berg, N. (2011). Democracy under uncertainty: The wisdom of crowds and the free-rider problem in group decision making. Psychological Review, 118(1), 7696.
  • Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., & Rhenius, D. (1999). Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(6), 15341555.
  • Laland, K. N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Learning & Behavior, 32(1), 414.
  • Latané, B., Williams, K., & Harkins, S. (1979). Many hands make light the work: The causes and consequences of social loafing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(6), 822832.
  • Laughlin, P. R., & Ellis, A. L. (1986). Demonstrability and social combination processes on mathematical intellective tasks. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 177189.
  • Lazer, D., & Friedman, A. (2007). The network structure of exploration and exploitation. Administrative Science Quarterly, 52, 667694.
  • Louwerse, M. M., Dale, R., Bard, E. G., & Jeuniaux, P. (2012). Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronized. Cognitive Science, 36(8), 14041426.
  • Lupyan, G., & Dale, R. (2010). Language structure is partly determined by social structure. PLoS ONE, 5, e8559. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0008559.
  • Mason, W. A., Jones, A., & Goldstone, R. L. (2008). Propagation of innovations in networked groups. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(3), 422433.
  • Mason, W. A., & Watts, D. J. (2012). Collaborative learning in networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 764769.
  • McElreath, R., Bell, A. V., Efferson, C., Lubell, M., Richerson, P. J., & Waring, T. (2008). Beyond existence and aiming outside the laboratory: Estimating frequency-dependent and pay-off-biased social learning strategies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 35153528.
  • McElreath, R., Lubell, M., Richerson, P., Waring, T., Baum, W., Edsten, E., Efferson, C., & Paciotti, B. (2005). Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(6), 483508.
  • Mesoudi, A. (2008). An experimental simulation of the ‘copy-successful-individuals’ cultural learning strategy: Adaptive landscapes, producer-scrounger dynamics and informational access costs. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 350363.
  • Rendell, L., Boyd, R., Cownden, D., Enquist, M., Eriksson, K., Feldman, M. W., Fogarty, L., Ghirlanda, S., Lillicrap, T., & Laland, K. N. (2010). Why copy others? Insights from the social learning strategies tournament. Science, 328(5975), 208213.
  • Rogers, A. R. (1988). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90, 819831.
  • Rogers, E. M. (2003). Diffusion of innovations (5th ed.). New York: Free Press.
  • Rubinstein, A. (2003). ‘Economics and psychology’? The case of hyperbolic discounting. International Economic Review, 44 (4), 12071216.
  • Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychological Review, 63 (2), 129138.
  • Smith, J. W., Benkman, C. W., & Coffey, K. (1999). The use and misuse of public information. Behavioral Ecology, 10, 5462.
  • Sumpter, D. J. T. (2010). Collective animal behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  • Templeton, J. J., & Giraldeau, L.-A. (1996). Vicarious sampling: the use of personal and public information by starlings foraging in a simple patchy environment. Behavorial Ecology and Sociobiology, 38, 105114.
  • Theiner, G, Allen, C., & Goldstone, R. L. (2010). Recognizing group cognition. Cognitive Systems Research, 11, 378395.
  • Tomasello, M. (1994). The question of chimpanzee culture. In R. Wrangham, W. McGrew, F. de Waal & P. Heltne (Eds.), Chimpanzee cultures (pp. 301317). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Wisdom, T. N., & Goldstone, R. L. (2011). Imitation, innovation, and problem-solving in a networked group. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences, 15, 229252.
  • Zentall, T. R. (2003). Imitation in animals: How do they do it? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 9195.
    Direct Link: