SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Bailey, T.M., & Plunkett, K. (2002). Phonological specificity in early words. Cognitive Development, 17 (2), 12651282.
  • Ballem, K.D., & Plunkett, K. (2005). Phonological specificity in children at 1; 2. Journal of Child Language, 32 (1), 159173.
  • Banse, R., & Scherer, K.R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70 (3), 614636.
  • Best, C.T., Levitt, A., & McRoberts, G.W. (1991). Examination of language-specific influences in infants’ discrimination of prosodic categories. Proceedings of the XlIth lnternational Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Vol. 4 (pp. 162165). Aix-en-Provence: Universite de Provence.
  • Bijeljac-Babic, R., Serres, J., Hohle, B., & Nazzi, T. (2012). Effect of bilingualism on lexical stress pattern discrimination in French-learning infants. PLoS ONE, 7 (2), e30843.
  • Birch, S., & Clifton, C. (2002). Effects of varying focus and accenting of adjuncts on the comprehension of utterances. Journal of Memory and Language, 47 (4), 571588.
  • Bosch, L., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (1997). Native-language recognition abilities in 4-month-old infants from monolingual and bilingual environments. Cognition, 65 (1), 3369.
  • Braun, B., & Johnson, E.K. (2011). Question or tone 2? How language experience and linguistic function guide pitch processing. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 585594.
  • Chao, Y.R. (1948). Mandarin primer: An intensive course in spoken Chinese. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  • Cooper, R.P., & Aslin, R.N. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61 (5), 15841595.
  • Coulson, S., & Federmeier, K.D. (in press). Words in context: ERPs and the lexical/postlexical distinction. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.
  • Crystal, T.H., & House, A.S. (1988). Segmental durations in connected-speech signals: current results. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 83 (4), 15531573.
  • Curtin, S. (2010). Young infants encode lexical stress in newly encountered words. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 376385.
  • Curtin, S., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J.F. (2011). Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 492504.
  • Dietrich, C., Swingley, D., & Werker, J.F. (2007). Native language governs interpretation of salient speech sound differences at 18 months. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 104, 454464.
  • Fennell, C.T., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J.F. (2007). Using speech sounds to guide word learning: the case of bilingual infants. Child Development, 78 (5), 15101525.
  • Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 8 (2), 181195.
  • Fernald, A., & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 10 (3), 279293.
  • Fromkin, V. (1978). Tone: A linguistic survey. New York: Academic Press.
  • Gandour, J.T. (1977). On the interaction between tone and vowel length: evidence from Thai dialects. Phonetica, 34 (1), 5465.
  • Gandour, J.T. (1978). The perception of tone. In V.A. Fromkin (Ed.), Tone: A linguistic survey (pp. 4176). New York: Academic Press.
  • Gandour, J.T. (1984). Tone dissimilarity judgments by Chinese listeners. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 12, 235261.
  • Gandour, J., Dzemidzic, M., Wong, D., Lowe, M., Tong, Y., Hsieh, L., & Lurito, J. (2003). Temporal integration of speech prosody is shaped by language experience: an fMRI study. Brain and Language, 84 (3), 318336.
  • Golinkoff, R.M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Cauley, K.M., & Gordon, L. (1987). The eyes have it: lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 14 (1), 2345.
  • Hay, J., Wang, T., & Saffran, J. (2012, November). Perceptual narrowing in the second year: The case of pitch contour. Paper presented at Boston University Conference on Lanugage Development, Boston, MA.
  • Hohle, B., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Herold, B., Weissenborn, J., & Nazzi, T. (2009). Language specific prosodic preferences during the first half year of life: evidence from German and French. Infant Behavior and Development, 32, 262274.
  • Hollich, G. (2005). Supercoder: A program for coding preferential looking (Version 1.5). [Computer Software] West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University.
  • Howie, J.M. (1976). Acoustical studies of Mandarin vowels and tones. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Jongman, A., Wang, Y., Moore, C., & Sereno, J. (2006). Perception and production of Mandarin tone. In P. Li, L. H. Tan, E. Bates & O.J.L. Tzeng (Eds.), Handbook of East Asian psycholinguistics (Vol. 1: Chinese) (pp. 209217). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Karlsson, A., House, D., & Svantesson, J. (2012). Intonation adapts to lexical tone: the case of Kammu. Phonetica, 69, 2847.
  • Kitamura, C., & Burnham, D. (2003). Pitch and communicative intent in mother's speech: adjustments for age and sex in the first year. Infancy, 4 (1), 85110.
  • Kitamura, C., & Notley, A. (2009). The shift in infant preferences for vowel duration and pitch contour between 6 and 10 months of age. Developmental Science, 12 (5), 706714.
  • Kuhl, P.K., Conboy, B.T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., & Nelson, T. (2008). Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: new data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 9791000.
  • Kuhl, P.K., Williams, K.A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K.N., & Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science, 255 (5044), 606608.
  • Lee, C.Y., Tao, L., & Bond, Z.S. (2008). Identification of acoustically modified Mandarin tones by native listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 36 (4), 537563.
  • Lieberman, P. (1967). Intonation, perception and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Mani, N., Coleman, J., & Plunkett, K. (2008). Phonological specificity of vowel contrasts at 18-months. Language and Speech, 51 (1–2), 321.
  • Mani, N., & Plunkett, K. (2007). Phonological specificity of vowels and consonants in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 57 (2), 252272.
  • Mani, N., & Plunkett, K. (2008). Fourteen-month-olds pay attention to vowels in novel words. Developmental Science, 11 (1), 5359.
  • Mani, N., & Plunkett, K. (2010). Twelve-month-olds know their cups from their keps and tups. Infancy, 15 (5), 445470.
  • Mattock, K., & Burnham, D. (2006). Chinese and English infants’ tone perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization. Infancy, 10 (3), 241265.
  • Mattock, K., Molnar, M., Polka, L., & Burnham, D. (2008). The developmental course of lexical tone perception in the first year of life. Cognition, 106 (3), 13671381.
  • Namy, L.L. (2001). What's in a name when it isn't a word? 17-month-olds’ mapping of non-verbal symbols to object categories. Infancy, 2, 7386.
  • Namy, L.L., & Waxman, S.R. (1998). Words and gestures: infants’ interpretations of different forms of symbolic reference. Child Development, 69 (2), 295308.
  • Nazzi, T. (2005). Use of phonetic specificity during the acquisition of new words: differences between consonants and vowels. Cognition, 98 (1), 1330.
  • Nazzi, T., & New, B. (2007). Beyond stop consonants: consonantal specificity in early lexical acquisition. Cognitive Development, 22 (2), 271279.
  • Ohala, J.J. (1978). The production of tone. In V.A. Fromkin (Ed.), Tone: A linguistic survey (pp. 539). New York: Academic Press.
  • Pegg, J.E., Werker, J.F., & McLeod, P.J. (1992). Preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech: evidence from 7-week-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 15 (3), 325345.
  • Pike, K.L. (1945). Tone languages: The nature of tonemic systems, with a technique for the analysis of their significant pitch contrasts. University of Virginia: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
  • Polka, L., Colantonio, C., & Sundara, M. (2001). Cross-language comparison of /d/–/ð/ perception: evidence for a new developmental pattern. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109 (5), 21902200.
  • Quam, C., & Swingley, D. (2010). Phonological knowledge guides two-year-olds’ and adults’ interpretation of salient pitch contours in word learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 62 (2), 135150.
  • Ramon-Casas, M., Swingley, D., Sebastian-Galles, N., & Bosch, L. (2009). Vowel categorization during word recognition in bilingual toddlers. Cognitive Psychology, 59 (1), 96121.
  • Remez, R.E., Fellowes, J.M., & Rubin, P.E. (1997). Talker identification based on phonetic information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 651666.
  • Schachner, A., & Hannon, E.E. (2011). Infant-directed speech drives social preferences in 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 47 (1), 1925.
  • Sebastian-Galles, N., Echeverria, S., & Bosch, L. (2005). The influence of initial exposure on lexical representation: comparing early and simultaneous bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 240255.
  • Sebastian-Galles, N., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Diego-Balaguer, R., & Diaz, B. (2006). First- and second-language phonological representations in the mental lexicon. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 12771291.
  • Singh, L., & Foong, J. (2012). Influences of lexical tone and pitch on word recognition in bilingual infants. Cognition, 124 (2), 128142.
  • Singh, L., Morgan, J.L., & White, K.S. (2004). Preference and processing: the role of speech affect in early spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 173189.
  • Singh, L., White, K.S., & Morgan, J.L. (2008). Building a phonological lexicon in the face of variable input: effects of pitch and amplitude variation on early word recognition. Language Learning and Development, 4, 157178.
  • Skoruppa, K., Cristia, A., Peperkamp, S., & Seidl, A. (2011). English-learning infants’ perception of word stress patterns. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130 (1), EL50EL55.
  • Skoruppa, K., Pons, F., Bosch, L., Christophe, A., Cabrol, D., & Peperkamp, S. (2013). The development of word stress perception in French and Spanish infants. Language Learning and Development, 9 (1), 88104.
  • Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, J.C. (2003). Using prosody to avoid ambiguity: effects of speaker awareness and referential context. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 103130.
  • So, C.K., & Best, C.T. (2010). Cross-language perception of non-native tonal contrasts: effects of native phonological and phonetic influences. Language and Speech, 53 (Pt 2), 273293.
  • Sommers, M.S., Nygaard, L.C., & Pisoni, D.B. (1992). Stimulus variability and the perception of spoken words: effects of variation in speaking rate and overall amplitude. Proceedings 1992 International Conference on Spoken Language Processing. Banff, Canada.
  • Swingley, D. (2003). Phonetic detail in the developing lexicon. Language and Speech, 46 (Pt 2–3), 265294.
  • Swingley, D., & Aslin, R.N. (2000). Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children. Cognition, 76 (2), 147166.
  • Swingley, D., & Aslin, R.N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science, 13 (5), 480484.
    Direct Link:
  • Van Heuven, V.J., & Haan, J. (2002). Temporal development of interrogativity cues in Dutch. In C. Gussenhoven & N. Warner (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology VII (pp. 6186). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Werker, J.F., & Tees, R.C. (1983). Developmental changes across childhood in the perception of non-native speech sounds. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 37 (2), 278286.
  • Werker, J.F., & Tees, R.C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7 (1), 4963.
  • White, K.S., & Morgan, J.L. (2008). Sub-segmental detail in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 59 (1), 114132.
  • Woodward, A.L., & Hoyne, K.L. (1999). Infants’ learning about words and sounds in relation to objects. Child Development, 70, 6577.
  • Xu, Y. (1999). Effects of tone and focus on the formation and alignment of f0 contours. Journal of Phonetics, 27 (1), 55105.
  • Yeung, H.H., Chen, K.H., & Werker, J.F. (2013). When does native language input affect phonetic perception? The precocious case of lexical tone. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 123139.
  • Yip, M.J.W. (2002). Tone. Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Yoshida, K.A., Fennell, C.T., Swingley, D., & Werker, J.F. (2009). Fourteen-month-old infants learn similar-sounding words. Developmental Science, 12 (3), 412418.