You have free access to this content

Developmental Science

Cover image for Vol. 18 Issue 1

January 2015

Volume 18, Issue 1

Pages i–ii, 1–182

  1. ISSUE INFORMATION

    1. Top of page
    2. ISSUE INFORMATION
    3. DEVELOPMENTAL SCIENCE REVIEW
    4. PAPERS
    5. SHORT REPORT
    6. PAPER
    7. SHORT REPORTS
    1. You have free access to this content
      Issue Information (pages i–ii)

      Article first published online: 28 DEC 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12252

  2. DEVELOPMENTAL SCIENCE REVIEW

    1. Top of page
    2. ISSUE INFORMATION
    3. DEVELOPMENTAL SCIENCE REVIEW
    4. PAPERS
    5. SHORT REPORT
    6. PAPER
    7. SHORT REPORTS
    1. You have free access to this content
      Development and the epigenome: the ‘synapse’ of gene–environment interplay (pages 1–23)

      W. Thomas Boyce and Michael S. Kobor

      Article first published online: 28 DEC 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12282

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      Epigenetic marks—chromatin structural modifications that regulate gene expression without changing DNA sequences—may offer a strong, parsimonious account for the convergence of genetic and contextual variation in the genesis of adaptive and maladaptive development. DNA methylation and histone acetylation, for example, can change the ‘loose’ or ‘tight’ conformation of chromatin, which is arrayed within chromosomes like beads on a string.

  3. PAPERS

    1. Top of page
    2. ISSUE INFORMATION
    3. DEVELOPMENTAL SCIENCE REVIEW
    4. PAPERS
    5. SHORT REPORT
    6. PAPER
    7. SHORT REPORTS
    1. You have full text access to this OnlineOpen article
      Visual motherese? Signal-to-noise ratios in toddler-directed television (pages 24–37)

      Sam V. Wass and Tim J. Smith

      Article first published online: 7 APR 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12156

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      Younger brains are noisier information processing systems; this means that information for younger individuals has to allow clearer differentiation between those aspects that are required for the processing task in hand (the ‘signal’) and those that are not (the ‘noise’). We compared toddler-directed and adult-directed TV programmes (TotTV/ATV). We examined how low-level visual features (that previous research has suggested influence gaze allocation) relate to semantic information, namely the location of the character speaking in each frame.

    2. You have free access to this content
      Young children ‘solve for x’ using the Approximate Number System (pages 38–49)

      Melissa M. Kibbe and Lisa Feigenson

      Article first published online: 3 MAR 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12177

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      The Approximate Number System (ANS) supports basic arithmetic computation in early childhood, but it is unclear whether the ANS also supports the more complex computations introduced later in formal education. ‘Solving for x’ in addend-unknown problems is notoriously difficult for children, who often struggle with these types of problems well into high school. Here we asked whether 4–6-year-old children could solve for an unknown addend using the ANS.

    3. You have full text access to this OnlineOpen article
      Training-induced recovery of low-level vision followed by mid-level perceptual improvements in developmental object and face agnosia (pages 50–64)

      Maria Lev, Sharon Gilaie-Dotan, Dana Gotthilf-Nezri, Oren Yehezkel, Joseph L. Brooks, Anat Perry, Shlomo Bentin, Yoram Bonneh and Uri Polat

      Article first published online: 4 APR 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12178

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      Abnormal visual inputs during development can impair various visual functions, and it is unclear whether these can be corrected during adulthood. Here, visual training at the age of 20 significantly improved LG's underdeveloped basic and mid-level visual functions with long-term persistence in trained and also untrained visual functions.

    4. You have free access to this content
      Words, shape, visual search and visual working memory in 3-year-old children (pages 65–79)

      Catarina Vales and Linda B. Smith

      Article first published online: 11 APR 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12179

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      Do words cue children's visual attention, and if so, what are the relevant mechanisms? Across four experiments, 3-year-old children were tested in visual search tasks in which targets were cued with only a visual preview versus a visual preview and a spoken name. The experiments were designed to determine whether labels facilitated search times and to examine one route through which labels could have their effect: By influencing the visual working memory representation of the target. The results show that labels modulate the encoding of the target in working memory, which in turn influences the processing of visual information.

    5. You have free access to this content
      Surprise! Infants consider possible bases of generalization for a single input example (pages 80–89)

      LouAnn Gerken, Colin Dawson, Razanne Chatila and Josh Tenenbaum

      Article first published online: 7 APR 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12183

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      Infants have been shown to generalize from a small number of input examples. However, existing studies allow two possible means of generalization. One is via a process of noting similarities shared by several examples. Alternatively, generalization may reflect an implicit desire to explain the input. The latter view suggests that generalization might occur when even a single input example is surprising, given the learner's current model of the domain.

    6. You have free access to this content
      Electrophysiological evidence of heterogeneity in visual statistical learning in young children with ASD (pages 90–105)

      Shafali S. Jeste, Natasha Kirkham, Damla Senturk, Kyle Hasenstab, Catherine Sugar, Chloe Kupelian, Elizabeth Baker, Andrew J. Sanders, Christina Shimizu, Amanda Norona, Tanya Paparella, Stephanny F.N. Freeman and Scott P. Johnson

      Article first published online: 13 MAY 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12188

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      We investigated the electrophysiological correlates of visual statistical learning in young children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) using an event-related potential shape learning paradigm, and we examined the relation between visual statistical learning and cognitive function. Compared to typically developing (TD) controls, the ASD group as a whole showed reduced evidence of learning as defined by N1 (early visual discrimination) and P300 (attention to novelty) components. Upon further analysis, in the ASD group there was a positive correlation between N1 amplitude difference and non-verbal IQ, and a positive correlation between P300 amplitude difference and adaptive social function. Children with ASD and a high non-verbal IQ and high adaptive social function, therefore, demonstrated a distinctive pattern of learning. This is the first study to identify electrophysiological markers of visual statistical learning in children with ASD, and the first to demonstrate heterogeneity in statistical learning in ASD that maps onto non-verbal cognition and adaptive social function.

    7. You have free access to this content
      Neurocognitive mechanisms of learning to read: print tuning in beginning readers related to word-reading fluency and semantics but not phonology (pages 106–118)

      Aleksandra K. Eberhard-Moscicka, Lea B. Jost, Margit Raith and Urs Maurer

      Article first published online: 26 MAY 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12189

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      This study demonstrates the presence of print tuning in the first year of reading acquisition and its development at the individual level. Moreover, individual differences in print tuning are not only related to word-reading fluency, but also to semantic knowledge.

    8. You have free access to this content
      Tracing children's vocabulary development from preschool through the school-age years: an 8-year longitudinal study (pages 119–131)

      Shuang Song, Mengmeng Su, Cuiping Kang, Hongyun Liu, Yuping Zhang, Catherine McBride-Chang, Twila Tardif, Hong Li, Weilan Liang, Zhixiang Zhang and Hua Shu

      Article first published online: 24 JUN 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12190

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      “In this 8-year longitudinal study, we traced the vocabulary growth of Chinese children, explored potential precursors of vocabulary knowledge, and investigated how vocabulary growth predicted future reading skills. Three subgroups of lexical growth were classified, namely high-high (with a large initial vocabulary size and a fast growth rate), low-high (with a small initial vocabulary size and a fast growth rate) and low-low (with a small initial vocabulary size and a slow growth rate) groups. Low-high and low-low groups were distinguishable mostly through phonological skills, morphological skills and other reading-related cognitive skills. Childhood vocabulary development (using intercept and slope) explained subsequent reading skills. Findings suggest that language-related and reading-related cognitive skills differ among groups with different developmental trajectories of vocabulary, and the initial size and growth rate of vocabulary may be two predictors for later reading development.”

    9. You have free access to this content
      Knowledge cannot explain the developmental growth of working memory capacity (pages 132–145)

      Nelson Cowan, Timothy J. Ricker, Katherine M. Clark, Garrett A. Hinrichs and Bret A. Glass

      Article first published online: 18 JUN 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12197

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      The article addresses the question of whether the improvement in working memory capacity across the elementary school years can be explained by the increase in knowledge. The figure shows a score based on mean items in working memory in a recognition test, normalized across all four age groups. These z scores were nearly identical for English letters that embody prior knowledge and for unfamiliar characters, disproving the view that working memory development is based completely on knowledge development. The results exclude a subset of young children who did not remember at least one English letter per trial, showing that knowledge also is important.

  4. SHORT REPORT

    1. Top of page
    2. ISSUE INFORMATION
    3. DEVELOPMENTAL SCIENCE REVIEW
    4. PAPERS
    5. SHORT REPORT
    6. PAPER
    7. SHORT REPORTS
    1. You have full text access to this OnlineOpen article
      Sentence repetition is a measure of children's language skills rather than working memory limitations (pages 146–154)

      Marianne Klem, Monica Melby-Lervåg, Bente Hagtvet, Solveig-Alma Halaas Lyster, Jan-Eric Gustafsson and Charles Hulme

      Article first published online: 1 JUL 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12202

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      Sentence repetition tasks are widely used in diagnosis and assessment of children with language impairments, but the underlying abilities measured are poorly understood, and how performance on this test should be interpreted is unclear. By investigating the longitudinal relationship between sentence repetition and other measures of language abilities in children aged 4-6 years, our findings support the view that sentence repetition is best seen as a reflection of an underlying unitary language construct, rather than as a measure of a separate construct with a specific role in language processing.

  5. PAPER

    1. Top of page
    2. ISSUE INFORMATION
    3. DEVELOPMENTAL SCIENCE REVIEW
    4. PAPERS
    5. SHORT REPORT
    6. PAPER
    7. SHORT REPORTS
    1. You have free access to this content
      Why the body comes first: effects of experimenter touch on infants' word finding (pages 155–164)

      Amanda Seidl, Ruth Tincoff, Christopher Baker and Alejandrina Cristia

      Article first published online: 16 APR 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12182

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      The lexicon of 6-month-olds is comprised of names and body part words. Unlike names, body part words do not often occur in isolation in the input. This presents a puzzle: How have infants been able to pull out these words from the continuous stream of speech at such a young age? We hypothesize that caregivers' interactions directed at and on the infant's body may be at the root of their early acquisition of body part words.

  6. SHORT REPORTS

    1. Top of page
    2. ISSUE INFORMATION
    3. DEVELOPMENTAL SCIENCE REVIEW
    4. PAPERS
    5. SHORT REPORT
    6. PAPER
    7. SHORT REPORTS
    1. You have free access to this content
      Cross-cultural investigation into cognitive underpinnings of individual differences in early arithmetic (pages 165–174)

      Maja Rodic, Xinlin Zhou, Tatiana Tikhomirova, Wei Wei, Sergei Malykh, Victoria Ismatulina, Elena Sabirova, Yulia Davidova, Maria Grazia Tosto, Jean-Pascal Lemelin and Yulia Kovas

      Article first published online: 26 JUN 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12204

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      The present study assessed 626 5-7-year-old children in the UK, China, Russia, and Kyrgyzstan on a cognitive test battery measuring: (1) general skills; (2) non-symbolic number sense; (3) symbolic number understanding; (4) simple arithmetic - operating with numbers; and (5) familiarity with numbers. Although most inter-population differences were small, 13% of the variance in arithmetic skills could be explained by the sample, replicating the pattern, previously found with older children in PISA. Despite average differences, the same cognitive skills were related to early arithmetic in these diverse populations.

    2. You have free access to this content
      Causal learning from probabilistic events in 24-month-olds: an action measure (pages 175–182)

      Anna Waismeyer, Andrew N. Meltzoff and Alison Gopnik

      Article first published online: 16 JUL 2014 | DOI: 10.1111/desc.12208

      Thumbnail image of graphical abstract

      Using a two-choice action measure, we tested whether 24-month-olds can use observed probabilistic information to solve a causal learning problem in the absence of causal linguistic descriptions or spatial contact. Toddlers first observed an adult produce a probabilistic pattern of causal evidence and were then given an opportunity to design their own intervention. At test, toddlers used the observed probabilistic causal information to generate their own intervention to bring about the same effect on the world.

SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION