SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

Keywords:

  • Brain anatomy;
  • Chinese;
  • COMT rs4680;
  • FreeSurfer;
  • healthy;
  • hippocampus;
  • population difference;
  • volume;
  • working memory;
  • young adults

Abstract

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Material and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments

Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val158Met (rs4680) polymorphism plays a crucial role in regulating brain dopamine level. Converging evidence from Caucasian samples showed that, compared with rs4680 Val allele, the Met allele was linked to lower COMT activity, which in turn was linked to better cognitive performance such as working memory (WM) and to a larger hippocampus (a brain region important for WM). However, some behavioral studies have shown that the function of rs4680 appears to vary across different ethnic groups, with Chinese subjects showing an opposite pattern as that for Caucasians (i.e. the Val allele is linked to better cognitive functions related to WM in Chinese). Using a sample of healthy Han Chinese college students (ages from 19 to 21 years), this study investigated the association of COMT Val158Met genotype with behavioral data on a two-back WM task (n = 443, 189M/254F) and T1 MRI data (n = 320, 134M/186F). Results showed that, compared to the Met allele, the Val allele was associated with larger hippocampal volume (the right hippocampus: β = −0.118, t = −2.367, P = 0.019, and the left hippocampus: β = −0.099, t = −1.949, P = 0.052) and better WM performance (β = −0.110, t = −2.315, P = 0.021). These results add to the growing literature on differentiated effects of COMT rs4680 polymorphism on WM across populations and offer a brain structural mechanism for such population-specific genetic effects.

Working memory (WM) refers to the retention of information in conscious awareness (Postle 2006), which is essential for cognitive functions such as reading, learning, decision making and planning (Baddeley et al. 1986; Engle et al. 1999). In the past decade, many studies have been conducted to examine the genetic and neural bases of WM.

Several molecular studies have identified the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene as playing an important role in WM. COMT is located on Chr. 22q and codes for COMT enzyme that metabolizes catecholamines and catechol-estrogens. A G/A substitution at codon 158 in COMT, commonly known as Val158Met or rs4680, causes an alteration from valine (Val) to methionine (Met), consequently modulates the activity of COMT (Chen et al. 2004; Lachman et al. 1996; Tunbridge et al. 2006). Catechol-O-methyltransferase acts as a chief modulator of dopamine level in the central nervous system and periphery (Lachman 2008). It has been shown that, compared to the Val allele, COMT 158Met allele is related to increased tonic and reduced phasic dopamine release in the subcortical regions but increased dopamine levels in the cortex (Bilder et al. 2004). Because dopamine is one of the most pivotal neurotransmitters in the brain and plays an important role in WM processing (Castner et al. 2000; Mcnab et al. 2009), COMT val158met polymorphism is related to brain development and cognitive functions such as executive function and WM (Leh et al. 2010; Lindenberger et al. 2008).

Interestingly, however, the associations between rs4680 and WM or related cognitive performances have been found to vary by population. Several studies with Caucasian samples (Aguilera et al. 2008; Bruder et al. 2005) indicated that the Met allele is associated with better cognitive function, especially WM, but in a study of Chinese, Yeh et al. (2009) found that Val carriers performed better in all basic competency test subtests than the Met homozygotes. Yeh et al.'s finding has been supported by many other studies of Asian samples on the associations between COMT and cognitive functions (Cheon et al. 2008; Ma et al. 2007; Qian et al. 2009; Tai & Wu 2002; Wu et al. 2001; Zhang et al. 2007). For example, a study of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder children (Qian et al. 2009) reported that the patients with high-enzymatic activity (Val/Val) of COMT Val158Met performed significantly better on an intelligence quotient test than did patients with mid- or low-level enzymatic activity (Val/Met and Met/Met). In another study, female Met carriers were found to perform significantly poorer on the Freedom from Distractibility task than the other genotype groups (Zhang et al. 2007). Such research evidence has led researchers to speculate on the reasons for ethnic differences in gene–behavior associations (Lee & Ham 2008).

One plausible reason for ethnic differences in genetic effects might be physiological in nature – perhaps COMT is linked to brain anatomy subserving cognitive functions differently for different groups. It has been widely agreed that the hippocampus plays an essential role in WM, especially when relational encoding process is involved. Neuroimaging studies (Berent-Spillson et al. 2010; Bokde et al. 2010; Nakao et al. 2002; Stepanichev et al. 2004; Zarahn et al. 2005) indicated that bilateral hippocampi were specifically activated during WM processing. In addition, brain lesion studies have also reported that patients with hippocampal damage had difficulty retaining information just presented (Ezzyat & Olson 2008; Olson et al. 2006). These results suggest that the hippocampus is critical for WM and some researchers have even deemed its structure as an endophenotype for WM performance (Axmacher et al. 2010; Ranganath & D'esposito 2005; Wang & Cai 2010). Furthermore, researchers have documented that COMT expression is widely distributed in the hippocampus (Aalto et al. 2005; Bertolino et al. 2008; Gill & Mizumori 2006; Li et al. 2010; Seamans et al. 1998; Takahashi et al. 2008; Wilkerson & Levin 1999). Several studies on Caucasian samples have found that the Met allele is associated with larger hippocampal volumes (Cerasa et al. 2008; Dutt et al. 2009; Ehrlich et al. 2010; Honea et al. 2009). However, there have been only two structural imaging genetics studies of rs4680 in Chinese subjects and they focused on the effect of rs4680 on white matter connectivity and integrity (Li et al. 2009; Liu et al. 2010). Neither examined the association between COMT and hippocampal volumes. To fill that void, this study aimed to examine the association between the COMT gene, hippocampal volume and WM in a large sample of healthy young Chinese.

Material and methods

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Material and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments

Participants

Four hundred and forty-six (191 male and 255 female, ages from 19 to 21 years) healthy Han Chinese college students were recruited from Beijing Normal University for this study. Demographic information including age, gender and handedness was collected (see Table 1). All participants reported having normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Data based on the Beck Depression Inventory showed that none of the participants met the criterion for major depression. Participants also reported having had no history of psychiatric diseases, head injuries or stroke/seizure. This experiment was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning at Beijing Normal University, China. Written consent form was obtained from each participant after a full explanation of the study procedure.

Table 1. Demographic statistics for different genotypes
VariablesVal/ValVal/MetMet/MetF or χ2P
  1. M/F, male/female.

Subjects with data on the WM task (N = 443)     
Group size24816827  
Gender (M/F)113/13566/10210/17χ 2 = 1.990.37
Age (mean ± SD in year)20.45 ± 0.8620.32 ± 0.8620.47 ± 1.00F = 1.130.32
Handedness (% of right hand)93.5592.8692.59F = 0.030.97
Education (mean in year)14.514.514.5F = 0.001.00
Subjects with data from MRI 3D T1-image (N = 320)
Group size17312819  
Gender (M/F)81/9248/805/14χ 2 = 4.640.10
Age (mean ± SD in year)20.46 ± 0.8920.34 ± 0.8920.56 ± 0.92F = 0.830.44
Handedness (% of right hand)97.1197.65100F = 0.330.72
Education (mean in year)14.514.514.5F = 0.001.00

Material and cognitive tasks

A two-back WM paradigm with three subtasks (semantic, phonological and morphological judgment) was used to assess WM performance (for details, see Li et al. 2011). Briefly, participants were asked to make continuous judgments between the word currently presented and the word presented two trials earlier in terms of their semantic (whether they belong to the same semantic category), phonological (whether they rhymed with each other) or morphological relations (whether they were the same Tibetan character) (Xue et al. 2004). Data from three participants were excluded because they had too many invalid and missing trials, resulting in a final sample of 443 subjects (189 males and 254 females) in the gene–behavior analysis. Factor analysis was conducted for the three WM tasks and the first principal component was extracted to represent WM ability.

Genotyping

Genotyping was performed as described in a previous article (Chen et al. 2011). The genomic DNA for each participant was extracted from a 4-ml venous blood sample according to the standard method. The polymerase chain reaction method was used to genotype rs4680. Samples with ambiguous or unidentifiable results were re-amplified and re-scored. A random subset of the samples (10%) were selected and tested twice for confirmation. Among all 443 subjects, 248 were Val/Val, 168 were Val/Met and 27 were Met/Met. The sample was in Hardy–Weinberg Equilibrium (χ2 = 0.024, P = 0.877). The minor allele frequency of this single nucleotide polymorphism in our sample was 0.25, which is comparable to the HapMap data (29% for Han Chinese in Beijing and 26% for Chinese in Metropolitan, Denver, Colorado, according to HapMap Genome Browser release #28).

MRI data collection and analysis

Three hundred and twenty (186 females and 134 males) of the subjects participated in the MRI experiment. Three-dimensional MRI scans were performed with a 3.0T Siemens Magnetom Trio scanner (Tim Trio, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with a standard head coil at Beijing Normal University Brain Imaging Center. Structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data were acquired with the T1-weighted MPRAGE pulse sequence (TE = 3.75 milliseconds, TR = 2530 milliseconds, flip angle = 7°; FOV = 256 × 256 mm2, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1.33 mm3, number of partitions = 128).

MRI data were analyzed automatically with atlas-based FreeSurfer segmentation software (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu, version 4.5.0) to extract volumetric measures of regions of interest (Fischl et al. 2002). Volumes of bilateral hippocampi were generated according to the standard FreeSurfer segmentation and parcellation procedures, relying upon variations in voxel signal intensities, probabilistic atlas location and local spatial relationships between the structures (Fischl et al. 2002). Intracranial volume (ICV), including brain tissues and other biological materials such as meninges and cerebrospinal fluid, was taken from the standard output of FreeSurfer analysis as well. Quality control of scan images and segmentation was assured by visual inspection of the whole cortex of each subject, and any inaccuracies in Talairach transformation, skull stripping and segmentation were manually corrected, and re-inspected. Manual corrections included editing gray matter surface for most subjects to exclude membrane tissues, editing white matter surface to add control points in FreeSurfer and obtaining good segmentation of the hippocampus.

Statistical analysis

Linear regression models were used to detect the associations between rs4680 genotype and WM performance and bilateral hippocampal volumes. The 443 subjects were separated into the Val/Val group (n = 248, with135 females, coded as 1), the Val/Met group (n = 168, with 102 females, coded as 2) and the Met/Met group (n = 27, with 17 females, coded as 3). As head sizes vary significantly between individuals, ICV and gender were included as covariates of no interest as well. In addition, gender-by-genotype interaction was also included to detect gender differences in genetic effects. Because of the small sample size of the Met homozygotes, we also reanalyzed the data by combining the heterozygotes and Met homozygotes into the Met carriers group. Statistical analysis was carried out in SPSS16.0 for Windows.

Results

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Material and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments

Demographic statistics

The three genotype groups did not differ in any of the four demographic variables (age, gender, years of education and handedness, see Table 1).

Working memory performance

Working memory performance was computed through a principle component analysis based on the three WM subtasks (Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy: 0.627; Bartlett's test of sphericity: χ2 (df = 3) = 108.24; P < 0·001). The first factor explained 53.39% of the total variance, and the z-score of each WM subtask accuracy loaded almost equally on the factor: 0.71 for semantic WM, 0.74 for phonological WM and 0.74 for morphological WM. No significant gender difference was found for WM performance: mean and SD were −0.04 ± 0.98 for males and 0.03 ± 1.02 for females, F (1, 441) = 0.648, P = 0.421.

Working memory and hippocampal volume

The relationships between WM and bilateral hippocampal volumes were examined by partial correlation analysis controlling for the effects of gender and ICV. The partial correlation coefficient was not significant for either the left hemisphere [r (316) = 0.08, P = 0.153] or the right hemisphere [r (316) = 0.101, P = 0.071].

Gene–behavior association

Regression analysis with rs4680 genotype as the independent variable and gender as the covariate revealed a significant genotype difference in WM performance (Table 2). Gender was not a significant covariate, β = 0.045, t = 0.958, P = 0.338. COMT's effect was significant, β = −0.110, t = −2.315, P = 0.021, with the number of Met allele negatively associated with WM performance: the Val/Val group showed the best WM performance, the Met/Met group the worst performance and the heterozygotes in-between performance (Fig. 1). To examine whether COMT showed a sexual dimorphic effect, gender-by-genotype interaction was also added and it was not significant, β = 0.054, t = 0.265, P = 0.791. Because of the small sample size of the Met/Met group, we also reanalyzed the data by combining this group with the heterozygotes to form the Met carriers group. In this way, the genetic effect of COMT on WM was also significant, β = −0.095, t = −1.990, P = 0.047.

Table 2. Regression models
RegressorICVGenderCOMT rs4680Model summary
 βTPβTPβTPdf1df2FP
  1. HPC, hippocampus; WM, working memory; ICV, intracranial volume.

  2. a

    Marginal significance.

  3. b

    P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001.

WM performance   0.0450.9580.338−0.110−2.3150.021*24403.0060.050a
Left HPC volume0.4266.758<0.001***0.0010.0220.982−0.099−1.9490.052a331626.219<0.001***
Right HPC volume0.4256.875<0.001***−0.038 −0.611 0.542−0.118−2.3670.019*331631.018<0.001***
image

Figure 1. WM factor scores of the three genotypes. The Val/Val individuals (N = 248, with 113 males) scored significantly higher than the Met/Val group (N = 168, with 66 males) and the Met/Met group (N = 27, with 10 males) scored lowest on the WM tasks (factor scores, mean ± SE). Error bar indicates the standard error of the mean. *P value of rs4680 as a regressor less than 0.05.

Download figure to PowerPoint

Gene–brain association

Regression analyses showed that, after controlling for gender and ICV, the dose effect of COMT rs4680 polymorphism on right hippocampal volume was significant (β = −0.118, t = −2.367, P = 0.019, Val/Val > Val/Met > Met/Met), and was marginally significant on left hippocampal volume, β = −0.099, t = −1.949, P = 0.052 (see Table 2; Fig. 2). The gender-by-genotype interaction was not significant for either the left (β = −0.135, t = −0.582, P = 0.561) or the right hippocampus (β = −0.223, t = −0.983, P = 0.326). Again because of the small sample size of the Met/Met group, we also reanalyzed the data by combining this group with the heterozygotes to form the Met carriers group. Results showed that the genetic effect was significant for the right hippocampal volume (β = −0.117, t = −2.352, P = 0.019), but not significant for the left hippocampal volume (β = −0.082, t = −1.606, P = 0.109). Finally, to examine whether COMT might have a more generalized effect on the whole brain, we analyzed its association with ICV. Regression results (with gender included as a control variable) showed that ICV did not differ by COMT rs4680 genotype (β = −0.036, t = −0.795, P = 0.427).

image

Figure 2. Bilateral hippocampal volumes (mean ±SE) of the Val/Val (N= 173, with 81 males), Val/Met (N= 128, with 48 males) and Met/Met (N= 19, with 5 males) groups. Error bar indicates the standard error of the mean. *P value of rs4680 as a regressor less than 0.05.

Download figure to PowerPoint

Discussion

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Material and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments

Working memory has been found to show considerable heritability (Ando et al. 2001), indicating its substantial genetic basis. Previous studies have reported the contributions of dopamine-related genes to WM (Barnett et al. 2008; Bertolino et al. 2006; Li et al. 2011; Markett et al. 2010; Parasuraman et al. 2005; Wilkosc et al. 2010). The COMT val158met polymorphism (i.e. rs4680) regulates the activity of COMT enzyme directly in the form of dose effect of gene expression: the number of Val allele is positively associated with activity of COMT enzyme (Syvanen et al. 1997).

This study reported significant associations of rs4680 polymorphism with WM performance and right hippocampal volume (and a marginal association with left hippocampal volume). The number of Val alleles was positively associated with WM, namely, Val homozygotes performed better than heterozygotes, followed by Met homozygotes. Further two-group (Val homozygotes vs. Met carriers) analysis showed that participants with the Val allele (high activity of COMT function) in our Chinese sample performed better on the two-back WM task than the Met allele carriers. These results are in clear contrast with previous reports based on Caucasian samples (Aguilera et al. 2008; Bruder et al. 2005; Farrell et al. 2012; Kennedy et al. 2011), but are consistent with previous studies of Asian samples using tasks related to WM (Cheon et al. 2008; Ma et al. 2007; Qian et al. 2009; Tai & Wu 2002; Wu et al. 2001; Yeh et al. 2009; Zhang et al. 2007). Similarly, our finding that the Val allele rather than the Met allele was associated with larger hippocampi also contradicted the finding from studies of Caucasians (Cerasa et al. 2008; Honea et al. 2009). No such study has been conducted with Asian samples. Taken together, these results suggest the existence of a systematic ethnic difference in the role of COMT rs4680 in WM, which may have been due to ethnic differences in COMT's effect on WM-related brain structure. It is worth noting that, the effect size of COMT appeared to be larger on brain structure than on WM. This might suggest a closer connection between genes and the brain than between genes and behavior. Alternatively, it may have been due to unsatisfactory measurement of behaviors than that of brain structure. Perhaps hippocampal volume is a reliable endophenotype of WM. Finally, it should be mentioned that COMT's effect on brain structure may be direct or indirect. As mentioned earlier, COMT directly modulates dopamine levels in the hippocampus. Indirectly, dopamine is associated with several neurotrophic factors including BDNF (brain-derived neurotrophic factor) (Kuppers & Beyer 2001), which promotes the development of cerebral cortex (e.g. enlarging the brain area and increasing the cortical thickness) (Ho et al. 2007).

Several factors need to be considered when discussing possible explanations of ethnic differences in genetic effects. First, allele frequencies vary greatly across ethnic populations. The frequency of low activity allele (Met) of rs4680 in Caucasians is about 0.5: for example, 0.50 in a study by Kunuqi et al. (1997), 0.42 in Cerasa's study (Cerasa et al. 2008) and 0.48 in Utah residents with Northern and Western European ancestry from the CEPH (Centre d'Etude du Polymorphisme Humain) collection of the HapMap group. In contrast, the Met allele's frequency is much lower in Asian populations: 0.29 in a Japanese sample (Kunugi et al. 1997), 0.25 in our sample and 0.29 for Han Chinese in Beijing and 0.26 for Han Chinese in Denver, Colorado, in the HapMap data.

The most likely explanation of population-specific genetic effects is gene–gene interaction. A number of genes are involved in the dopamine system and they may interact with the COMT gene. For example, there is evidence that Monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) pathway might compensate for blockade of COMT pathway of monoamine catabolism (Eisenhofer & Finberg 1994). MAOA also varies greatly in allele frequencies across ethnic groups. The same is true for other enzymes such as tyrosine hydroxylase, dopa decarboxylase and dopamine transporter. Future research needs to explore the gene–gene interactions and to see whether they would explain ethnic differences in genetic effects (Munafo et al. 2008).

In summary, our results indicate that COMT val158met polymorphism was significantly associated with WM performance and hippocampal volume in a Chinese sample. Direction of the genetic effect was opposite of that found in Caucasian samples, suggesting ethnic differences in COMT–hippocampus–WM association.

References

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Material and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments
  • Aalto, S., Bruck, A., Laine, M., Nagren, K. & Rinne, J.O. (2005) Frontal and temporal dopamine release during working memory and attention tasks in healthy humans: a positron emission tomography study using the high-affinity dopamine D2 receptor ligand [11C]FLB 457. J Neurosci 25, 24712477.
  • Aguilera, M., Barrantes-Vidal, N., Arias, B., Moya, J., Villa, H., Ibanez, M.I., Ruiperez, M.A., Ortet, G. & Fananas, L. (2008) Putative role of the COMT gene polymorphism (Val158Met) on verbal working memory functioning in a healthy population. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 147B, 898902.
  • Ando, J., Ono, Y. & Wright, M.J. (2001) Genetic structure of spatial and verbal working memory. Behav Genet 31, 615624.
  • Axmacher, N., Lenz, S., Haupt, S., Elger, C.E. & Fell, J. (2010) Electrophysiological signature of working and long-term memory interaction in the human hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci 31, 177188.
  • Baddeley, A., Logie, R., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S. & Spinnler, H. (1986) Dementia and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol A 38, 603618.
  • Barnett, J.H., Scoriels, L. & Munafo, M.R. (2008) Meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene Val158/108Met polymorphism. Biol Psychiatry 64, 137144.
  • Berent-Spillson, A., Persad, C.C., Love, T., Tkaczyk, A., Wang, H., Reame, N.K., Frey, K.A., Zubieta, J.K. & Smith, Y.R. (2010) Early menopausal hormone use influences brain regions used for visual working memory. Menopause 17, 692699.
  • Bertolino, A., Blasi, G., Latorre, V., Rubino, V., Rampino, A., Sinibaldi, L., Caforio, G., Petruzzella, V., Pizzuti, A., Scarabino, T., Nardini, M., Weinberger, D.R. & Dallapiccola, B. (2006) Additive effects of genetic variation in dopamine regulating genes on working memory cortical activity in human brain. J Neurosci 26, 39183922.
  • Bertolino, A., Di Giorgio, A., Blasi, G., Sambataro, F., Caforio, G., Sinibaldi, L., Latorre, V., Rampino, A., Taurisano, P., Fazio, L., Romano, R., Douzgou, S., Popolizio, T., Kolachana, B., Nardini, M., Weinberger, D.R. & Dallapiccola, B. (2008) Epistasis between dopamine regulating genes identifies a nonlinear response of the human hippocampus during memory tasks. Biol Psychiatry 64, 226234.
  • Bilder, R.M., Volavka, J., Lachman, H.M. & Grace, A.A. (2004) The catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism: relations to the tonic-phasic dopamine hypothesis and neuropsychiatric phenotypes. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 19431961.
  • Bokde, A.L., Karmann, M., Born, C., Teipel, S.J., Omerovic, M., Ewers, M., Frodl, T., Meisenzahl, E., Reiser, M., Moller, H.J. & Hampel, H. (2010) Altered brain activation during a verbal working memory task in subjects with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis 21, 103118.
  • Bruder, G.E., Keilp, J.G., Xu, H., Shikhman, M., Schori, E., Gorman, J.M. & Gilliam, T.C. (2005) Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) genotypes and working memory: associations with differing cognitive operations. Biol Psychiatry 58, 901907.
  • Castner, S.A., Williams, G.V. & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (2000) Reversal of antipsychotic-induced working memory deficits by short-term dopamine D1 receptor stimulation. Science 287, 20202022.
  • Cerasa, A., Gioia, M.C., Labate, A., Liguori, M., Lanza, P. & Quattrone, A. (2008) Impact of catechol-O-methyltransferase Val(108/158) Met genotype on hippocampal and prefrontal gray matter volume. Neuroreport 19, 405408.
  • Chen, J., Lipska, B.K., Halim, N., Ma, Q.D., Matsumoto, M., Melhem, S., Kolachana, B.S., Hyde, T.M., Herman, M.M., Apud, J., Egan, M.F., Kleinman, J.E. & Weinberger, D.R. (2004) Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am J Hum Genet 75, 807821.
  • Chen, C., Moyzis, R., Dong, Q., He, Q., Zhu, B., Li, J., Li, H. & Lessard, J. (2011) Sex modulates the associations between the COMT gene and personality traits. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 15931598.
  • Cheon, K.A., Jun, J.Y. & Cho, D.Y. (2008) Association of the catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism with methylphenidate response in a classroom setting in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 23, 291298.
  • Dutt, A., McDonald, C., Dempster, E., Prata, D., Shaikh, M., Williams, I., Schulze, K., Marshall, N., Walshe, M., Allin, M., Collier, D., Murray, R. & Bramon, E. (2009) The effect of COMT, BDNF, 5-HTT, NRG1 and DTNBP1 genes on hippocampal and lateral ventricular volume in psychosis. Psychol Med 39, 17831797.
  • Ehrlich, S., Morrow, E.M., Roffman, J.L., Wallace, S.R., Naylor, M., Bockholt, H.J., Lundquist, A., Yendiki, A., Ho, B.C., White, T., Manoach, D.S., Clark, V.P., Calhoun, V.D., Gollub, R.L. & Holt, D.J. (2010) The COMT Val108/158Met polymorphism and medial temporal lobe volumetry in patients with schizophrenia and healthy adults. Neuroimage 53, 9921000.
  • Eisenhofer, G. & Finberg, J.P. (1994) Different metabolism of norepinephrine and epinephrine by catechol-O-methyltransferase and monoamine oxidase in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 268, 12421251.
  • Engle, R.W., Kane, M.J. & SW, T. (1999) Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In Miyake, A. & Shah, P. (eds), Models of Working Memory. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 102134.
  • Ezzyat, Y. & Olson, I.R. (2008) The medial temporal lobe and visual working memory: comparisons across tasks, delays, and visual similarity. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 8, 3240.
  • Farrell, S.M., Tunbridge, E.M., Braeutigam, S. & Harrison, P.J. (2012) COMT Val(158)Met genotype determines the direction of cognitive effects produced by catechol-O-methyltransferase inhibition. Biol Psychiatry 71, 538544.
  • Fischl, B., Salat, D.H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., van der Kouwe, A., Killiany, R., Kennedy, D., Klaveness, S., Montillo, A., Makris, N., Rosen, B. & Dale, A.M. (2002) Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33, 341355.
  • Gill, K.M. & Mizumori, S.J. (2006) Context-dependent modulation by D(1) receptors: differential effects in hippocampus and striatum. Behav Neurosci 120, 377392.
  • Ho, B.C., Andreasen, N.C., Dawson, J.D. & Wassink, T.H. (2007) Association between brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met gene polymorphism and progressive brain volume changes in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 164, 18901899.
  • Honea, R., Verchinski, B.A., Pezawas, L., Kolachana, B.S., Callicott, J.H., Mattay, V.S., Weinberger, D.R. & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2009) Impact of interacting functional variants in COMT on regional gray matter volume in human brain. Neuroimage 45, 4451.
  • Kennedy, Q., Taylor, J.L., Noda, A., Adamson, M., Murphy, G.M. Jr., Zeitzer, J.M. & Yesavage, J.A. (2011) The roles of COMT val158met status and aviation expertise in flight simulator performance and cognitive ability. Behav Genet 41, 700708.
  • Kunugi, H., Nanko, S., Ueki, A., Otsuka, E., Hattori, M., Hoda, F., Vallada, H.P., Arranz, M.J. & Collier, D.A. (1997) High and low activity alleles of catechol-O-methyltransferase gene: ethnic difference and possible association with Parkinson's disease. Neurosci Lett 221, 202204.
  • Kuppers, E. & Beyer, C. (2001) Dopamine regulates brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) expression in cultured embryonic mouse striatal cells. Neuroreport 12, 11751179.
  • Lachman, H.M. (2008) Does COMT val158met affect behavioral phenotypes: yes, no, maybe? Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 30273029.
  • Lachman, H.M., Papolos, D.F., Saito, T., Yu, Y.M., Szumlanski, C.L. & Weinshilboum, R.M. (1996) Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics 6, 243250.
  • Lee, B.T. & Ham, B.J. (2008) Serotonergic genes and amygdala activity in response to negative affective facial stimuli in Korean women. Genes Brain Behav 7, 899905.
  • Leh, S.E., Petrides, M. & Strafella, A.P. (2010) The neural circuitry of executive functions in healthy subjects and Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 7085.
  • Li, J., Yu, C., Li, Y., Liu, B., Liu, Y., Shu, N., Song, M., Zhou, Y., Zhu, W., Li, K. & Jiang, T. (2009) COMT val158met modulates association between brain white matter architecture and IQ. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 150B, 375380.
  • Li, B., Arime, Y., Hall, F.S., Uhl, G.R. & Sora, I. (2010) Impaired spatial working memory and decreased frontal cortex BDNF protein level in dopamine transporter knockout mice. Eur J Pharmacol 628, 104107.
  • Li, J., Chen, C., He, Q., Li, H., Moyzis, R.K., Xue, G. & Dong, Q. (2011) Neurotensin receptor 1 gene (NTSR1) polymorphism is associated with working memory. PLoS One 6, e17365.
  • Lindenberger, U., Nagel, I.E., Chicherio, C., Li, S.C., Heekeren, H.R. & Backman, L. (2008) Age-related decline in brain resources modulates genetic effects on cognitive functioning. Front Neurosci 2, 234244.
  • Liu, B., Li, J., Yu, C., Li, Y., Liu, Y., Song, M., Fan, M., Li, K. & Jiang, T. (2010) Haplotypes of catechol-O-methyltransferase modulate intelligence-related brain white matter integrity. Neuroimage 50, 243249.
  • Ma, X., Sun, J., Yao, J., Wang, Q., Hu, X., Deng, W., Sun, X., Liu, X., Murray, R.M., Collier, D.A. & Li, T. (2007) A quantitative association study between schizotypal traits and COMT, PRODH and BDNF genes in a healthy Chinese population. Psychiatry Res 153, 715.
  • Markett, S.A., Montag, C. & Reuter, M. (2010) The association between dopamine DRD2 polymorphisms and working memory capacity is modulated by a functional polymorphism on the nicotinic receptor gene CHRNA4. J Cogn Neurosci 22, 19441954.
  • McNab, F., Varrone, A., Farde, L., Jucaite, A., Bystritsky, P., Forssberg, H. & Klingberg, T. (2009) Changes in cortical dopamine D1 receptor binding associated with cognitive training. Science 323, 800802.
  • Munafo, M.R., Brown, S.M. & Hariri, A.R. (2008) Serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and amygdala activation: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry 63, 852857.
  • Nakao, K., Ikegaya, Y., Yamada, M.K., Nishiyama, N. & Matsuki, N. (2002) Hippocampal long-term depression as an index of spatial working memory. Eur J Neurosci 16, 970974.
  • Olson, I.R., Page, K., Moore, K.S., Chatterjee, A. & Verfaellie, M. (2006) Working memory for conjunctions relies on the medial temporal lobe. J Neurosci 26, 45964601.
  • Parasuraman, R., Greenwood, P.M., Kumar, R. & Fossella, J. (2005) Beyond heritability: neurotransmitter genes differentially modulate visuospatial attention and working memory. Psychol Sci 16, 200207.
    Direct Link:
  • Postle, B.R. (2006) Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience 139, 2338.
  • Qian, Q.J., Wang, Y.F., Yang, L., Li, J., Guan, L.L., Chen, Y., Ji, N. & Liu, L. (2009) Association study of intelligence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder children in China. Beijing Da Xue Xue Bao 41, 285290.
  • Ranganath, C. & D'Esposito, M. (2005) Directing the mind's eye: prefrontal, inferior and medial temporal mechanisms for visual working memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15, 175182.
  • Seamans, J.K., Floresco, S.B. & Phillips, A.G. (1998) D1 receptor modulation of hippocampal-prefrontal cortical circuits integrating spatial memory with executive functions in the rat. J Neurosci 18, 16131621.
  • Stepanichev, M.Y., Zdobnova, I.M., Zarubenko, I.I., Moiseeva, Y.V., Lazareva, N.A., Onufriev, M.V. & Gulyaeva, N.V. (2004) Amyloid-beta(25–35)-induced memory impairments correlate with cell loss in rat hippocampus. Physiol Behav 80, 647655.
  • Syvanen, A.C., Tilgmann, C., Rinne, J. & Ulmanen, I. (1997) Genetic polymorphism of catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): correlation of genotype with individual variation of S-COMT activity and comparison of the allele frequencies in the normal population and Parkinsonian patients in Finland. Pharmacogenetics 7, 6571.
  • Tai, C.H. & Wu, R.M. (2002) Catechol-O-methyltransferase and Parkinson's disease. Acta Med Okayama 56, 16.
  • Takahashi, H., Kato, M., Takano, H., Arakawa, R., Okumura, M., Otsuka, T., Kodaka, F., Hayashi, M., Okubo, Y., Ito, H. & Suhara, T. (2008) Differential contributions of prefrontal and hippocampal dopamine D(1) and D(2) receptors in human cognitive functions. J Neurosci 28, 1203212038.
  • Tunbridge, E.M., Harrison, P.J. & Weinberger, D.R. (2006) Catechol-o-methyltransferase, cognition, and psychosis: Val158Met and beyond. Biol Psychiatry 60, 141151.
  • Wang, G.W. & Cai, J.X. (2010) Hippocampal-prefrontal cortical circuit and working memory. Dongwuxue Yanjiu 31, 5056.
  • Wilkerson, A. & Levin, E.D. (1999) Ventral hippocampal dopamine D1 and D2 systems and spatial working memory in rats. Neuroscience 89, 743749.
  • Wilkosc, M., Hauser, J., Tomaszewska, M., Dmitrzak-Weglarz, M., Skibinska, M., Szczepankiewicz, A. & Borkowska, A. (2010) Influence of dopaminergic and serotoninergic genes on working memory in healthy subjects. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 70, 8694.
  • Wu, R.M., Cheng, C.W., Chen, K.H., Lu, S.L., Shan, D.E., Ho, Y.F. & Chern, H.D. (2001) The COMT L allele modifies the association between MAOB polymorphism and PD in Taiwanese. Neurology 56, 375382.
  • Xue, G., Dong, Q., Jin, Z. & Chen, C. (2004) Mapping of verbal working memory in nonfluent Chinese-English bilinguals with functional MRI. Neuroimage 22, 110.
  • Yeh, T.K., Chang, C.Y., Hu, C.Y., Yeh, T.C. & Lin, M.Y. (2009) Association of catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) polymorphism and academic achievement in a Chinese cohort. Brain Cogn 71, 300305.
  • Zarahn, E., Rakitin, B., Abela, D., Flynn, J. & Stern, Y. (2005) Positive evidence against human hippocampal involvement in working memory maintenance of familiar stimuli. Cereb Cortex 15, 303316.
  • Zhang, K., Zheng, Z., Gao, X., Li, J. & Zhang, F. (2007) Possible relationship between the COMT gene ValMet polymorphism and psychometric IQ in girls of the Qinba region in China. Neuropsychobiology 56, 98103.

Acknowledgments

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Material and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the 111 Project of the Ministry of Education of China (B07008) and National Natural Science Foundation of China (31100807 and 31200850).