SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

Keywords:

  • Cetacea;
  • culture;
  • cultural evolution;
  • gene–culture coevolution;
  • vocal dialects

Abstract

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES

Human evolution has clearly been shaped by gene–culture interactions, and there is growing evidence that similar processes also act on populations of non-human animals. Recent theoretical studies have shown that culture can be an important evolutionary mechanism because of the ability of cultural traits to spread rapidly both vertically, obliquely, and horizontally, resulting in decreased within-group variance and increased between-group variance. Here, we collate the extensive literature on population divergence in killer whales (Orcinus orca), and argue that they are undergoing ecological speciation as a result of dietary specializations. Although we cannot exclude the possibility that cultural divergence pre-dates ecological divergence, we propose that cultural differences in the form of learned behaviours between ecologically divergent killer whale populations have resulted in sufficient reproductive isolation even in sympatry to lead to incipient speciation. © 2012 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2012, 106, 1–17.


THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES

Recent scientific advances have revolutionized our understanding of how gene–culture coevolution has shaped and influenced human evolution (e.g. Richerson & Boyd, 2005; Laland, Odling-Smee & Myles, 2010). Traditionally, it was thought that products of culture (e.g. shelter, clothing, and tools) helped humans shield themselves from other selective pressures, thus slowing down the rate of human adaptive evolution considerably. However, most scientists now agree that culture can also have the opposite effect of increasing the rate at which many human traits evolve, as well as influencing the direction of selection acting on human populations (Richerson & Boyd, 2005; Hawks et al., 2007; Laland et al., 2010). For example, the cultural practice of cattle farming in some human populations drove the evolution of lactose tolerance in adults (Simoons, 1978; Richerson & Boyd, 2005; Laland et al., 2010), and humans from agricultural populations living on high-starch diets have more copies of the amylase gene (e.g. Perry et al., 2007).

Consequently, there has been a recent push for interdisciplinary projects that integrate archaeological and anthropological data with discoveries of human genetics and evolutionary theory to further our understanding of human evolution (Laland et al., 2010). We propose that similar processes are also acting on populations of non-human animals, and can have far greater evolutionary consequences than has previously been assumed. In this review we will use the example of the killer whale (Orcinus orca) to illustrate how cultures and behavioural traditions can drive population divergence even in the absence of geographic isolation.

ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES

As an alternative to the allopatric model that explains speciation as the result of geographic isolation (e.g. Coyne & Orr, 2004), the idea that reproductive isolation and ultimately speciation can result from ecologically-based divergent selection has recently received considerable attention (e.g. Rundle & Nosil, 2005; Schluter, 2009). This model is referred to as ecological speciation, and can apply to allopatric, parapatric, and sympatric populations, as long as divergent selection is the ultimate cause of both population differentiation and reproductive isolation (Rundle & Nosil, 2005). Strong evidence for ecological speciation has come from a variety of natural systems, laboratory experiments, comparative studies, and theoretical models (reviewed by Rundle & Nosil, 2005; Schluter, 2009).

Here, we evaluate evidence that the largest member of the highly diverse family Delphinidae, the killer whale, is currently undergoing ecological speciation. Although generally regarded as a single species (Committee on Taxonomy, 2009; but see LeDuc, Robertson & Pitman, 2008; Morin et al., 2010), numerous ecologically divergent O. orca lineages are recognized throughout its global distribution (three in the North Pacific, five around Antarctica, and at least two in the North Atlantic; see below for more details).

Rundle & Nosil (2005) list three components necessary for demonstrating ecological speciation: (1) an ecological source of divergent selection between populations; (2) a form of reproductive isolation; and (3) a genetic mechanism linking divergent selection to reproductive isolation. Here, we begin by summarizing the evidence for population divergence among O. orca and outline the sources of divergent selection that appear to be responsible for population divergence. We then discuss potential causes of reproductive isolation and suggest that cultural traditions and cultural inheritance play a central role. Finally, we argue that the deterministic mechanism linking divergent selection to reproduction in O. orca may be cultural rather than genetic.

EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES

Orcinus orca are second only to humans as the most widely distributed mammal on earth. They inhabit all the world's oceans, with the greatest densities found in temperate and polar regions (Ford, 2009). Killer whales are the top predators in many marine ecosystems. As a species, their diet includes over 140 species of fish, squid, mammals, reptiles, and birds, but different O. orca populations typically show a high degree of dietary specialization (Ford, 2009).

In areas of high marine productivity, two or more ecologically specialized lineages, or ecotypes, of O. orca often co-occur in sympatry. Ecotypes show differences in diet, movement patterns, pigmentation, behaviour, and morphology (Figs 1, 2), as well as in the size, stability, and composition of social groups (Heimlich & Boran, 1999; Baird, 2000; Barrett-Lennard & Heise, 2006; Ford, 2009; Ford & Ellis, 2012; see Table 1).

image

Figure 1. Known killer whale (Orcinus orca) ecotypes of the Northern Hemisphere. Note the size, pigmentation, and dorsal fin shape differences between the sexes (males left; females right), but also between ecotypes. Artwork by Uko Gorter.

Download figure to PowerPoint

image

Figure 2. Known killer whale (Orcinus orca) ecotypes of the Southern Hemisphere. Note the size, pigmentation, and dorsal fin shape differences between the sexes (males left; females right), but also between ecotypes. Artwork by Uko Gorter.

Download figure to PowerPoint

Table 1.  Divergent traits between identified killer whale (Orcinus orca) ecotypes
RegionEcotype comparisonDivergent traitsEstimated time since divergence (mtDNA)References
ABASD/FGMMPPSS
  1. AB, acoustic behaviour; AS, acoustic signals; D/F, diet and foraging behaviour; G, genetics; M, morphology; MP, movement/diving patterns; P, pigmentation; SS, social structure; P, inconclusive, but preliminary studies suggest divergence; ?, data not available.

NE PacificResidents vs transientsYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYes∼700 000 yearsBaird & Whitehead (2000); Deecke et al. (2005); Barrett-Lennard & Heise (2006); Morin et al. (2010); Pilot et al. (2010); Ford & Ellis (2012); Riesch & Deecke (2011)
 Residents vs offshores?YesYesYesYesYesYesYes>150 000 yearsBarrett-Lennard & Heise (2006); Dahlheim et al. (2008); Morin et al. (2010); Pilot et al. (2010); Ford & Ellis (2012); Ford et al. (2011)
 Transients vs offshoresYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYes∼700 000 yearsBarrett-Lennard & Heise (2006); Dahlheim et al. (2008); Morin et al. (2010); Pilot et al. (2010); Ford & Ellis (2012); Ford et al. (2011)
AntarcticaType A vs type B (pack ice)??PYes??YesP>150 000 yearsPitman & Ensor (2003); Morin et al. (2010); Pitman (2011)
 Type A vs type B (Gerlache)??PYes??YesP>150 000 yearsPitman & Ensor (2003); Morin et al. (2010); Pitman (2011)
 Type A vs type C??YesYesYesPYesYes>150 000 yearsPitman & Ensor (2003); Pitman et al. (2007); Morin et al. (2010)
 Type A vs type D??P?YesPYes??Pitman et al. (2011)
 Type B (pack ice) vs type B (Gerlache)??Yes????Yes?Pitman (2011)
 Type B (pack ice) vs type C??YesYesYesPYesYes∼150 000 yearsPitman & Ensor (2003); Pitman et al. (2007); Andrews et al. (2008); Morin et al. (2010); Pitman (2011)
 Type B (pack ice) vs type D??P?YesPYes??Pitman et al. (2011); Pitman (2011)
 Type B (Gerlache) vs type C??YesYesYesPYesYes∼150 000 yearsPitman & Ensor (2003); Pitman et al. (2007); Andrews et al. (2008); Morin et al. (2010); Pitman (2011)
 Type B (Gerlache) vs type D??P?YesPYes??Pitman et al. (2011); Pitman (2011)
 Type C vs type D????YesPYes??Pitman et al. (2011); Pitman (2011)
NE AtlanticGeneralists vs mammal-eaters??Yes?YesYesYes??Similäet al. (1996); Wolkers et al. (2007); Foote et al. (2009, 2011c); Morin et al. (2010)

Sympatric ecotype assemblages are currently reported from three different geographical regions: the eastern North Pacific; the eastern North Atlantic; and Antarctica. But there is also recent evidence for sympatric ecological divergence of O. orca from around New Zealand, the Russian Far East, the western North Atlantic, and the eastern tropical Pacific (Reeves et al., 2004; ranges shown in Fig. 3). The lack of evidence for additional killer whale ecotypes from other parts of the world's oceans is most likely a consequence of the limited research effort in these regions.

image

Figure 3. World map with approximate distributions of known sympatric killer whale (Orcinus orca) ecotypes in red (dark grey in print) and potential sympatric ecotypes in yellow (white in print).

Download figure to PowerPoint

Best studied are the three sympatric ecotypes inhabiting the waters of the eastern North Pacific, called offshores, residents, and transients (Heimlich & Boran, 1999; Baird, 2000; Barrett-Lennard & Heise, 2006; Ford & Ellis, 2012). Several geographically distinct populations exist for each ecotype (Barrett-Lennard & Heise, 2006). The resident ecotype, for example, consists of the southern resident population (∼88 individuals, URL 1, 2011; mainly in southern British Columbia and Washington State, but also in Oregon and California), the northern resident population (∼260 individuals, Ellis, Towers & Ford, 2011; mainly in northern British Columbia, but also in south-eastern Alaska and Washington State; Fig. 1), and the South Alaskan resident population (> 700 individuals, Matkin & Durban, 2011; from eastern South Alaska west to the Alaska Peninsula). Recent evidence suggests that there are additional resident-like populations near the Aleutian Islands (Matkin et al., 2007; > 1500 individuals, Matkin & Durban, 2011), in the Bering Sea (Hoelzel et al., 2007), and in the western North Pacific, around Kamchatka (Ivkovich et al., 2010). The transient ecotype is composed of the West Coast transient population (∼250 individuals, URL 2, 2011; Fig. 1), the Gulf of Alaska transient population (< 100 individuals, Matkin & Durban, 2011), and the AT1 transient population of Prince William Sound, Alaska (around seven individuals, Barrett-Lennard & Heise, 2006; Matkin et al., 2008), with further putative transient populations in the Bering Sea and around the Aleutian Islands. Residents specialize on fish, in particular on salmon (Oncorhynchus spp.), whereas transients prey on marine mammals and occasionally seabirds (Barrett-Lennard & Heise, 2006; Ford, 2009; Ford & Ellis, 2012). Offshores probably form a single population ranging from the Aleutian Islands to California. Their dietary spectrum and their preferred prey have yet to be fully described, but preliminary observations suggest that they also specialize on fish such as Pacific sleeper shark (Somniosus pacificus) and Pacific halibut (Hippoglossus stenolepis); (Jones, 2006; Krahn et al., 2007; Dahlheim et al., 2008; Ford & Ellis, 2012; Ford et al., 2011). In addition to dietary specialization, these three killer whale ecotypes also differ in a variety of other traits, including group size and the degree of philopatry (Table 1).

Research suggests that up to five sympatric O. orca ecotypes are found in Antarctic waters (25 000–27 000 individuals; Branch & Butterworth, 2001): type A, type B (with a large and small form), type C, and type D (Fig. 2; Pitman & Ensor, 2003; Pitman, 2011; Pitman et al., 2011). Again, there appears to be a general pattern of specialization on either marine mammals and birds or fish, but the dietary distinctions warrant further investigation (Pitman & Ensor, 2003; Andrews, Pitman & Ballance, 2008). Type-A killer whales appear to prey almost exclusively on Antarctic minke whales (Balaenoptera bonaerensis), whereas the large form of type B (the ‘pack-ice killer whale’) specializes on seals, and the small form (the ‘Gerlache killer whale’) hunts penguins (Pitman, 2011; Fig. 2). Type-C killer whales, on the other hand, specialize on Antarctic toothfish (Dissostichus mawsoni), and show what is probably the most pronounced morphological divergence within O. orca, with both sexes being considerably smaller than other ecotypes (Pitman et al., 2007; Pitman, 2011; Fig. 2; Table 1). Finally, a recent study by Pitman et al. (2011) described the sub-Antarctic type D, which clearly differs from all other Antarctic ecotypes in pigmentation and morphology (Fig. 2). However, the available data are insufficient to identify whether type D also has a specific dietary niche.

Around Iceland, Shetland, and Norway, O. orca have also been reported to diverge in trophic ecology, but again the actual dietary specializations are not yet clearly resolved (Fig. 2). Whereas some killer whales in the North Atlantic appear to specialize on marine mammals (Foote et al., 2009; Deecke et al., 2011), others specialize on fish (predominantly herring, Clupea harengus; Similä & Ugarte, 1993; Similä, Holst & Christensen, 1996). Stable isotope analysis of individuals with the same mitochondrial haplotype suggests that some feed at different trophic levels (Foote et al., 2009, 2011c; but see Morin et al., 2010), which could either point to a greater niche width for these populations or could be an indication that this dietary shift is relatively recent, and has not yet resulted in genetic differentiation. More research is warranted to resolve this important issue.

BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES

Culture has been defined in various ways over the past decades, but the most common definitions characterize it as population-level behavioural variation that can be transmitted vertically (from parent to offspring), obliquely (from any member of an older generation to any member of a younger generation), and horizontally (within the same generation) via social learning (Laland & Janik, 2006; Danchin & Wagner, 2008; Laland & Galef, 2009). After decades of scientific debate, there is little disagreement regarding the existence of cultures in non-human animals (but see Laland & Galef, 2009). Some of the most famous examples of animal cultures are tool-use in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, orangutans, Pongo spp., and bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops spp.), and songs in songbirds and humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae; Whiten et al., 1999; Krützen et al., 2005; Laland & Janik, 2006; Laland & Galef, 2009; Garland et al., 2011). Culturally transmitted behavioural diversity has also been described in O. orca, and comprises traits such as acoustic communication, social behaviour, and foraging strategies (Table 2).

Table 2.  Cultural traditions of killer whale (Orcinus orca) populations from around the globe
PhenomenonBehavioural contextGeographic regionReferences
Call dialectsAll behavioursNE PacificFord (1989, 1991); Yurk et al. (2002); Deecke et al. (2005); Ford & Ellis (2012)
Greeting ceremoniesSocial behaviourNE PacificOsborne (1986)
Whistle dialectsSocial behaviourNE PacificRiesch et al. (2006); Riesch & Deecke (2011)
Beach rubbingSocial behaviourNE PacificFord (1989); Ford, Ellis & Balcomb (2000)
Benthic foragingForaging behaviourSW PacificVisser (1999)
Carousel feedingForaging behaviourNE AtlanticSimilä & Ugarte (1993); Domenici et al. (2000); Nottestad & Similä (2001); Simon et al. (2005)
  NW PacificTarasyan et al. (2005)
Endurance–exhaustion huntingForaging behaviourAtlanticGuinet et al. (2007)
Exploitation of longline fishingForaging behaviourwidespreadSecchi & Vaske (1998); Nolan & Liddle (2000); Visser (2000)
Intentional strandingForaging behaviourSW AtlanticLopez & Lopez (1985); Hoelzel (1991)
  S Indian OceanGuinet (1991); Guinet & Bouvier (1995)
Wave-washingForaging behaviourSouthern OceanSmith et al. (1981); Visser et al. (2008); Pitman (2011); Pitman & Durban (2012)

The best example for such behavioural variation in O. orca comes from residents and transients off Alaska, British Columbia, and Washington State. These ecotypes have been studied intensively since the 1970s (e.g. Bigg, 1982), and research has documented a complete lack of dietary overlap between the two ecotypes (based on observational studies, stomach content analysis of stranded individuals, and stable isotope analysis of blubber biopsies). In over 40 years of study, no resident has ever been found to kill and consume marine mammal prey, and no transient has been documented to take fish (e.g. Ford et al., 1998; Saulitis et al., 2000; Herman et al., 2005; Dahlheim & White, 2010). These dietary specializations result in differences in social structure between residents and transients, with transient group sizes usually being much smaller than resident group sizes (Bigg et al., 1990; Baird & Dill, 1996; Ford & Ellis, 1999; Baird, 2000).

AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES

Killer whales produce three types of sounds: echolocation clicks are thought to function in orientation and prey detection, whereas pulsed calls and whistles are communicative signals (Ford, 1989; Thomsen, Franck & Ford, 2002). Although there is recent evidence for some universal acoustic signals (Rehn et al., 2011), the structure and frequency of use of most vocalizations differs strikingly between ecotypes. In the north-eastern Pacific, for example, transient mammal-hunting O. orca use echolocation very sparingly, whereas resident, sympatric salmon specialists, echolocate frequently (Barrett-Lennard, Ford & Heise, 1996). The same applies for pulsed calls (Deecke, Ford & Slater, 2005) and whistles (Riesch & Deecke, 2011): transients produce these almost exclusively in non-hunting contexts. Whereas salmon cannot detect O. orca sounds over significant distances, marine mammals have good underwater hearing, and exhibit anti-predator behaviour in response to transient calls (Deecke, Slater & Ford, 2002). Mammal-eating killer whales therefore probably reduce their vocal behaviour to avoid eavesdropping by potential prey, and similar behavioural patterns have recently been revealed in mammal-hunting O. orca in the North Atlantic (Deecke et al., 2011).

In addition to differences in usage, the structure of O. orca vocalizations also varies among populations and social groups. Differences in the frequency content and patterning of echolocation clicks are probably related to the nature of the echolocation task, and to prey hearing (Barrett-Lennard et al., 1996), and thus are directly related to feeding. Many pulsed calls and some whistles are highly stereotyped, and can easily be assigned to call or whistle types (Ford, 1989; Riesch, Ford & Thomsen, 2006). Variation in communicative sounds can be found on three levels. First, there is geographic variation among allopatric populations, because spatially separated populations differ in their repertoires of stereotyped pulsed calls, e.g. eastern North Pacific (Ford, 1989, 1991; Yurk et al., 2002; Deecke et al., 2005), western North Pacific (Filatova et al., 2004), Norway (Strager, 1995), and Antarctica (Richlen & Thomas, 2008). Allopatric populations also differ in their whistle repertoires (Riesch et al., 2006; Samarra et al., 2010). Second, sympatric and parapatric populations exhibit dialect variation because populations of different ecotypes with overlapping home ranges also do not share call or whistle types. The same is usually true for populations of the same ecotype with adjacent or partially overlapping home ranges (Moore et al., 1988; Ford, 1991; Riesch et al., 2006; Riesch, Ford & Thomsen, 2008; Riesch & Deecke, 2011). Third, there is often dialect variation among social groups within a population. The Northern resident population for example contains three acoustic clans (Ford, 1991). Members belonging to different clans do not share any call types but associate frequently. They do, however, share stereotyped whistles (Riesch et al., 2006, 2008). Call types shared within a clan typically show matriline-specific variation in call structure (Miller & Bain, 2000; Nousek et al., 2006; Deecke et al., 2010).

CULTURAL TRANSMISSION

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES

The central requirement of culture is that it must be transmitted through social learning (Laland & Janik, 2006; Danchin & Wagner, 2008, 2010; Laland & Galef, 2009). In O. orca, behavioural and genetic studies have provided evidence that vocal dialects are transmitted via vocal learning rather than genetic inheritance (reviewed in Rendell & Whitehead, 2001; Janik, 2009). There is further evidence that vocal learning is not limited to vertical transmission from mother to offspring, but also takes place horizontally between matrilines with similar and different dialects (Deecke, Ford & Spong, 2000; Janik, 2009; Weißet al., 2010). The specialized hunting techniques of certain killer whale populations, such as the intentional stranding observed at Península Valdés, Argentina, and in the Crozet Islands (Table 2), have also been suggested to be transmitted via social learning from generation to generation (Hoelzel, 1991; Guinet & Bouvier, 1995), although experimental data on this are not yet available.

GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES

Similar to various other large mammals, such as cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus; O'Brien et al., 1983), Antarctic fur seals (Arctocephalus gazella; Hoffman et al., 2011), or humans (Li & Durbin, 2011), killer whales are characterized by low worldwide genetic diversity, both in mitochondrial and microsatellite DNA (Hoelzel et al., 2002; Morin et al., 2010). Whereas such a reduction in genetic diversity is often the result of small effective population sizes and historical bottlenecks (Hoelzel et al., 2002), ‘cultural hitchhiking’ has been proposed as an additional hypothesis to explain low mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) diversity in matrilineal odontocetes such as O. orca (Whitehead, 1998; but see Mesnick et al, 1999).

Overall, there is no evidence for a correlation between categories of preferred dietary items and genotypes on a global scale, which suggests that local prey specializations had multiple independent origins (Hoelzel et al., 2002; Morin et al., 2010; Foote et al., 2011b). In fact, both Morin et al. (2010) and Foote et al. (2011b) suggest that, based on mtDNA analyses, killer whales might have repeatedly migrated between the North Pacific and the North Atlantic. According to this scenario, transients, who are the sister group to all other killer whales, became isolated following an early colonization event, and residents and offshores originate from more recent colonization events (Morin et al., 2010; Foote et al., 2011b). Ecological divergence of offshores, residents, and transients could therefore have occurred in either allopatry or sympatry. In contrast, the divergence of multiple Antarctic ecotypes is more likely to have occurred in sympatry than allopatry (LeDuc et al., 2008; Morin et al., 2010; Foote et al., 2011b).

Both mtDNA and nuclear DNA analyses confirm profound genetic differentiation of O. orca ecotypes, albeit to different degrees (e.g. Hoelzel, Dahlheim & Stern, 1998; Barrett-Lennard, 2000; Hoelzel et al., 2007; Morin et al., 2010; Pilot, Dahlheim & Hoelzel, 2010; Foote et al., 2011c). Mitochondrial DNA suggests relatively old divergence times between ecotypes, with the oldest (i.e. transients) having diverged ∼ 700 000 years or 40 000 generations ago (Morin et al., 2010; Foote et al., 2011b; Table 1). These estimates are comparable with divergence times between several evolutionarily young cetacean species [e.g. the divergence between the three right whale species (Eubalaena spp.), the divergence between the Ganges River and the Indus River dolphin species (Platanista spp.), or the divergence between the long-finned and short-finned pilot whale (Globicephala spp.) within Delphinidae; McGowen, Spaulding & Gatesy, 2009]. On the other hand, divergence between different populations of the same ecotype appears to be more recent (e.g. Hoelzel et al., 2007).

Population genetic patterns as revealed by microsatellite analyses are not as straightforward. In the eastern North Pacific, the genetic distance based on nuclear DNA is greatest between resident and transient ecotypes, and FST values (fixation index; Weir & Cockerham, 1984) clearly suggest some degree of reproductive isolation between the two (e.g. Hoelzel et al., 1998, 2007; Morin et al., 2010; Pilot et al., 2010). A similar pattern of reproductive isolation has been described between the Antarctic type-A killer whales and both type-B and -C ecotypes (LeDuc et al., 2008). On the other hand, genetic distances between other ecotypes are smaller, and interbreeding appears to occur more often, for example between eastern North Pacific transients and offshores, eastern North Pacific residents and offshores (Barrett-Lennard, 2000; Hoelzel et al., 2007; Morin et al., 2010; Pilot et al., 2010), or Antarctic type-B and -C ecotypes (LeDuc et al., 2008).

How can these contradictory population genetic patterns be explained? In over 35 years of study, no case of between-culture dispersal or immigration has been recorded (J.K.B. Ford and G. M. Ellis, unpubl. data), and genetic analyses do not provide unequivocal evidence for permanent between-ecotype dispersal (e.g. Hoelzel et al., 2007; Pilot et al., 2010). However, gene flow is likely to occur during rare temporary associations between individuals of different ecotypes (see discussions in Hoelzel et al., 2007; Pilot et al., 2010). As offspring from such mating would then be born into and raised by their mother's social group, we would not expect these matings to leave an mtDNA signature, whereas they will be detectable using nuclear markers.

As a consequence of long-term divergence and several fixed haplotype differences in mtDNA, recent studies have suggested different species or subspecies designations for certain killer whale ecotypes (LeDuc et al., 2008; Morin et al., 2010); however, based on the reported patterns of gene flow revealed by the analysis of nuclear markers, we are not convinced that there is currently enough unambiguous evidence to warrant the designation of different species or subspecies.

CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES

Recent studies have shown that pre-mating reproductive isolation can arise between different cultures even in the absence of genetic change, ultimately driving speciation (theoretical model, Gavrilets, 2004; conceptual studies, Danchin & Wagner, 2008, 2010). Some of the best empirical evidence for this comes from Darwin's finches (Geospiza spp.), for which song, a culturally inherited trait, is an important factor in species recognition and mate choice (e.g. Grant & Grant, 1996, 2009, 2010). Males usually learn the song from their fathers, but females prefer song types that differ from those of their fathers, creating an effective barrier against inbreeding (Grant & Grant, 1996). This means that a culturally inherited trait could play a crucial role in speciation by promoting genetic isolation on secondary contact (Grant & Grant, 1996, 2009, 2010). We suggest that, similar to the situation in Darwin's finches, divergent cultural evolution has led to pre-zygotic and, at the very least, non-genetic post-zygotic reproductive isolation among certain killer whale populations or ecotypes, although reproductive isolation is not complete (see also Heimlich & Boran, 1999; Baird, 2000).

There are at least four hypothetical mechanisms (both pre- and post-mating reproductive barriers) by which cultural divergence can lead to reproductive isolation in O. orca. First, culture could act through xenophobia if cultural imprinting is so strong that mating will not take place between individuals belonging to different cultural groupings (Danchin & Wagner, 2010). For O. orca, we propose that a mechanism of positive and negative sexual/cultural imprinting acts on two levels but in opposite directions, similar to that described for humans (Rantala & Marcinkowska, 2011). On the basal level, acoustic similarity via, for example, matrilineal vocal dialects within certain populations can act as an acoustic family badge that reveals genetic relatedness (e.g. Deecke et al., 2010). Negative assortative mating by dialect has been shown in some populations (Barrett-Lennard, 2000), and means that animals can reliably avoid mating with close maternal kin, and thus control and reduce inbreeding. On the higher level, other cultural characteristics (e.g. group behaviour, social structure, dietary specializations, or vocal differences between populations; reviewed in Barrett-Lennard & Heise, 2006; Ford & Ellis, 2012) could act as a cultural badge that promotes endogamy within the cultural grouping by determining who an individual socializes and mates with. This process could be reinforced by differences in habitat use that limit the potential for direct interaction between ecotypes (Guinet & Bouvier, 1995; discussion in Hoelzel et al., 1998). In songbirds, local dialects are often correlated with female acoustic preferences, and may lead to reproductive isolation and ultimately speciation (Price, 2008). Similar processes could be affecting O. orca mating patterns, but no direct data exist on traits influencing mate preferences or the strength of behavioural isolation in this species.

Second, cultural specializations could provide an effective barrier for between-culture dispersal by inducing temporary natural selection against immigrants (Nosil, Vines & Funk, 2005): potential dispersers would have to adjust behaviourally to the local culture, and learn the local foraging behaviours (Table 2), in order to survive and reproduce with members of that culture (Danchin & Wagner, 2008, 2010). Until such cultural matching is achieved, dispersing individuals would experience drastically reduced fitness. Contrary to traditional views on ecological speciation (Rundle & Nosil, 2005), the reduction in immigrant fitness in killer whales could be based on learned behaviours rather than genetic adaptations (see also discussion in Pilot et al., 2010). Whether O. orca are able to acquire non-natal cultural traits throughout their lifetime, or whether there is an age-specific imprinting phase, is not known. The failure to successfully reintroduce the long-term captive whale Keiko to the wild suggests that the ability to correctly assimilate cultural traditions could be age specific (Simon et al., 2009), but further research into this is clearly warranted.

Third, cultural differences can lead to post-zygotic barriers through neutral processes such as genetic drift, rather than selection (Coyne & Orr, 2004). This is particularly likely, given the small population size of most killer whale ecotypes (e.g. Hoelzel et al., 2007), which increases both the potential for non-selective changes in allele frequencies as well as the speed of reproductive isolation arising through genetic drift (Coyne & Orr, 2004).

Finally, cultural differences can lead to post-zygotic barriers via gene–culture coevolution, when cultural innovations change selective pressures acting on a population, eventually leading to population divergence (Danchin & Wagner, 2008; Laland et al., 2010). For example, if a single individual within a population invents a new behaviour that aids in exploiting a resource (e.g. intentional stranding, Lopez & Lopez, 1985; Guinet, 1991; Hoelzel, 1991; Guinet & Bouvier, 1995), this innovation can spread rapidly within the population within a single generation as a result of vertical and horizontal transmission via social learning. Hence, cultural transmission can rapidly decrease within-group variance, while simultaneously increasing between-group variance (Lehmann & Feldman, 2008). This has the potential to effectively change the selective pressures acting on cultural groups, so that cultural change could facilitate or pre-empt a genetic response, just as described for human enzymes (Simoons, 1978; Whitehead, 1998; Richerson & Boyd, 2005; Hawks et al., 2007; Perry et al., 2007; Lehmann & Feldman, 2008; Laland et al., 2010). In O. orca, differences in metabolism or respiratory and muscular systems are to be expected between different ecotypes (Foote et al., 2011a). For example, a preliminary anatomical analysis suggests that residents and transients differ in skull morphology, body size, and other features, probably as a result of selection for robustness in transients to successfully hunt marine mammals (cited in Reeves et al., 2004). Moreover, the physiological requirements needed to successfully perform the endurance–exhaustion technique described for O. orca foraging on tuna (Guinet et al., 2007) are likely to differ from those needed for foraging on salmon (Ford et al., 1998) or marine mammals (Miller, Shapiro & Deecke, 2010). Other genetic changes similar to those found in humans are likely, but they will have to be the focus of future functional genomic research. In particular, we propose that next generation sequencing approaches could help uncover genes that are the target of selection in different killer whale ecotypes (Foote et al., 2011a). Candidate genes for this could be identified in model organisms for which the entire genome has been sequenced (e.g. humans or mice).

Orcinus orca of different ecotypes and from different geographic regions successfully interbreed in captivity (Bowles, Young & Asper, 1988; Duffield et al., 1995), ruling out genetic incompatibility (i.e. hybrid inviability), at least on the most coarse level. However, with the scientific data available to us, we were unable to discern if other genetic incompatibilities (e.g. hybrid sterility) applied to offspring sired by parents of different ecotypes (but for an account of offspring sired by hybrid parents in captivity, see URL 3 2011). If killer whale ecotypes have already evolved certain co-adapted genes, reduced hybrid fitness, for example in the form of lower growth rates or a reduced ability to process certain food items, is possible, but would only manifest itself under natural conditions.

Orcinus orca appears to be a good example of behavioural isolation evolving far ahead of hybrid inviability or sterility (Coyne & Orr, 2004; for a discussion of this phenomenon in birds, see Price, 2008). However, in theory, matrilineal behavioural preferences may be ephemeral and reversible (see discussion in LeDuc et al., 2008). In fact, reversal of the observed processes of differentiation as a result of hybridization or homogenization have been observed between different ecotypes in other well-established model systems such as sticklebacks and cichlids (Taylor et al., 2006; Seehausen et al., 2008; Nosil, Harmon & Seehausen, 2009; Behm, Ives & Boughman, 2010), and there is also good evidence for gene flow between ecotypes/populations in O. orca (e.g. Hoelzel et al., 2007; Pilot et al., 2010). Nonetheless, recent studies have demonstrated that speciation is possible despite the presence of gene flow (e.g. Dieckmann & Doebeli, 1999; Via, 2009; de León et al., 2010). Clearly, different killer whale ecotypes fall into different positions along the continuum between panmixia and complete reproductive isolation, and none seem to have yet achieved full reproductive isolation. Because of this, O. orca provides an interesting case study into the factors that promote or constrain ecotype movement along the speciation continuum towards ecological speciation (e.g. Hendry, 2009; Nosil et al., 2009). For example, what would happen if certain populations fall below a critical population size, or if no individual of breeding age for one sex is available? Whether speciation will occur depends on whether divergence exceeds the ecotype/population extinction rate. We could be witnessing the early stages of an adaptive radiation of killer whales, whereby a variety of incipient species are beginning to exploit diverse ecological niches, or conversely, we could be looking at an old and continuing process by which new ecotypes periodically form and become extinct again (Barrett-Lennard, 2011).

CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES

Three incidents may help shed some light on this question. The first one is the report of three transient killer whales captured off British Columbia in 1970. For the first 75 days of captivity, all three individuals refused to eat the fish provided by their captors, which eventually resulted in death by starvation of one animal. Only after that event did the other two transients begin eating fish, but they immediately reverted back to foraging on marine mammals after their release back to the wild (Ford & Ellis, 1999). For two other transients from the same capture an interesting case of intercultural transmission occurred when they, after 24 days of self-induced starvation, were put in a pool with a Southern resident, and were both feeding on herring within hours after being passed fish by the resident (G.M. Ellis, unpubl. data). The third example stems from observations made following the ‘Exxon Valdez’ oil spill in 1989. After the spill, pod AT1 (a small, genetically distinct population of transients from Prince William Sound, Alaska) lost nine members (41%) of their social group, including several reproductive females, and an additional four males died after 2000 (Matkin et al., 2008). As no successful recruitment has taken place into this social group since 1984, the group size is now reduced to seven individuals, with only two reproductive females and one adult male (Matkin et al., 2008). One possibility enabling group survival would be for AT1 to join another transient population, for example the Gulf of Alaska transients; however, there is so far no indication of this happening (Matkin et al., 2008). Thus, it appears that cultural traditions in O. orca can be a strong reproductive isolation mechanism that is stable even in the face of potential population extinction.

CONCLUSION

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES

Our review highlights multiple trait divergences among O. orca, including population divergence in behaviour, pigmentation patterns, morphology, dietary specializations, and genetics. Although there are still many open questions concerning the degree of this divergence and the strength of reproductive isolation, the overall pattern that emerges strongly suggests that ecological speciation is the driving force behind global killer whale diversity. Dietary specialization is likely to have resulted in divergent selection between populations, thus fulfilling the first requirement for ecological speciation. Behavioural isolation resulting from sexual imprinting is the most likely candidate for a mechanism for reproductive isolation (the second requirement of Rundle & Nosil, 2005). Although the currently available data did not allow us to identify a genetic mechanism linking divergent selection to reproductive isolation, we argue that this is not strictly necessary. What is needed is a deterministic mechanism that links divergent selection to reproductive isolation. As an alternative to strict genetic inheritance, another heritable unit – culture – clearly provides this deterministic mechanism in killer whales.

An interesting alternative, however, could be that cultural divergence may pre-date ecological divergence in O. orca, meaning that they may not be undergoing ecological speciation per se, but rather ‘cultural speciation’, as outlined by Gavrilets (2004); a combination of cultural and ecological mechanisms is also possible. Future studies are needed to shed more light on which came first, cultural or ecological divergence.

Whereas O. orca are probably unusual in the extent to which culturally-driven selection has driven diversification, and ultimately speciation (e.g. there is no evidence so far that culture has led to evolutionary significant levels of reproductive isolation in humans), culture and behavioural traditions also appear to be important features in the biology of various other cetacean species, such as humpback whales, sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus), and bottlenose dolphins (reviewed in Rendell & Whitehead, 2001; Laland & Galef, 2009). Hence, the combination of cultural and ecological divergence may have been the main driving force behind some of the observed species diversity in the order Cetacea (Berta, Sumich & Kovacs, 2006). Most importantly, however, such divergence may not be restricted to cetaceans, but may be affecting any species in which socially transmitted behaviours have fitness consequences. Although most of the divergence took place in the past, killer whales provide an excellent opportunity to study patterns of divergence and diversification in action. In addition, these patterns of current ecotype diversity in O. orca also have important implications for conservation, because they raise questions about the evolutionary significant units that warrant protection (e.g. Moritz, 1994; Hoelzel, 1998; Ryan, 2006; Whitehead, 2010).

Because many behavioural traits, such as vocal dialects, can be easily quantified, O. orca provide an exceptional opportunity to investigate how cultures can affect the evolutionary trajectories of populations, an aspect often ignored in the conceptual literature on speciation processes (but see Coyne & Orr, 2004; Price, 2008). Recent studies have demonstrated that culture plays an important role in shaping human evolution via culture–gene coevolution (Simoons, 1978; Richerson & Boyd, 2005; Hawks et al., 2007; Perry et al., 2007; Laland et al., 2010). The evidence we provide for O. orca suggests that culture and behavioural traditions could have far greater evolutionary consequences than previously assumed (see also Heimlich & Boran, 1999; Baird, 2000). Following the example of Danchin & Wagner (2010), we therefore propose that the cultural component of behaviour should be included along with phenotypic plasticity and epigenetics in a revised form of the modern synthesis of the study of evolution (see also Pigliucci, 2007; Pennisi, 2008). Furthermore, we propose that the third component of ecological speciation be extended to include both genes and culture as mechanisms linking divergent selection and reproductive isolation.

Recent years have seen the emergence of more specific theoretical models on how ecologically-based divergent natural selection can result in population divergence, and ultimately speciation (e.g. Gavrilets et al., 2007; Sadedin et al., 2009). Future research on O. orca could – to the extent that this is possible with field studies on such a large marine organism – specifically test some of these models and their predictions.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES

We thank R.R. Dunn, R.A. Martin, R.B. Langerhans, and members of the Langerhans' laboratory, as well as M. Plath, I. Schlupp, and four anonymous reviewers for valuable comments and discussions that helped improve previous versions of this article. We are greatly indebted to Uko Gorter for allowing us to use his illustration of different O. orca ecotypes. S.E. Diamond helped create Figure 3. V.B.D. was supported by a Marie-Curie Intra-European Fellowship.

REFERENCES

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. THE ROLE OF CULTURE IN EVOLUTION
  4. ECOLOGICAL SPECIATION AND ORCINUS ORCA
  5. EVIDENCE FOR POPULATION DIVERGENCE AMONG KILLER WHALES
  6. BEHAVIOURAL DIVERGENCE AND KILLER WHALE CULTURES
  7. AN EXAMPLE OF CULTURE: VOCAL BEHAVIOUR IN ORCINUS ORCA
  8. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION
  9. GENETIC DIVERGENCE, ECOTYPE FORMATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION
  10. CULTURAL DIVERGENCE, TYPES OF REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION, AND SPECIATION
  11. CAN CULTURAL TRADITIONS LEAD TO STABLE REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION?
  12. CONCLUSION
  13. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
  14. REFERENCES
  • Andrews RD, Pitman RL, Ballance LT. 2008. Satellite tracking reveals distinct movement patterns for Type B and Type C killer whales in the southern Ross Sea, Antarctica. Polar Biology 31: 14611468.
  • Baird RW. 2000. The killer whale: foraging specializations and group hunting. In: Mann J, Connor RC, Tyack PJ, Whitehead H, eds. Cetacean societies: field studies of dolphins and whales. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 127153.
  • Baird RW, Dill LM. 1996. Ecological and social determinants of group size in transient killer whales. Behavioral Ecology 7: 408416.
  • Baird RW, Whitehead H. 2000. Social organization of mammal-eating killer whales: group stability and dispersal patterns. Canadian Journal of Zoology 78: 20962105.
  • Barrett-Lennard LG. 2000. Population structure and mating patterns of killer whales, Orcinus orca, as revealed by DNA analysis. Ph.D. thesis. Vancouver: University of British Columbia.
  • Barrett-Lennard LG, Ford JKB, Heise KA. 1996. The mixed blessing of echolocation: differences in sonar use by fish-eating and mammal-eating killer whales. Animal Behaviour 51: 553565.
  • Barrett-Lennard LG, Heise KA. 2006. The natural history and ecology of killer whales. In: Estes JA, DeMaster DP, Doak DF, Williams TM, Brownell J, eds. Whales, whaling and ocean ecosystems. Berkeley: University of California Press, 163173.
  • Barrett-Lennard L. 2011. Killer whale evolution: Populations, ecotypes, species, oh my! Journal of the American Cetacean Society 40: 4853.
  • Behm JA, Ives AR, Boughman JW. 2010. Breakdown of postmating isolation and the collapse of a species pair through hybridization. American Naturalist 175: 1126.
  • Berta A, Sumich JL, Kovacs KM. 2006. Marine mammals: evolutionary biology, 2nd edn. London: Academic Press.
  • Bigg MA. 1982. An assessment of killer whale (Orcinus orca) stocks off Vancouver Island, British Columbia. Report of the International Whaling Commission 32: 655666.
  • Bigg MA, Olesiuk PF, Ellis GM, Ford JKB, Balcomb KC. 1990. Social organization and genealogy of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in the coastal waters of British Columbia and Washington State. In: Hammond PS, Donovan GP, eds. Individual recognition of cetaceans: use of photo-identification and other techniques to estimate population parameters. Reports of the International Whaling Commission, special issue 12. Cambridge: International Whaling Commission, 383405.
  • Bowles AE, Young WG, Asper ED. 1988. Ontogeny of stereotyped calling of a killer whale calf Orcinus orca during her first year. Rit Fiskideilar 11: 251175.
  • Branch TA, Butterworth DS. 2001. Estimates of abundance south of 60°S for cetacean species sighted frequently on the 1978/79 to 1997/98 IWC/IDCR- SOWER sighting surveys. Journal of Cetacean Research and Management 3: 251270.
  • Committee on Taxonomy. 2009. List of marine mammal species and subspecies. Society for Marine Mammalogy. Available at: http://www.marinemammalscience.org consulted on 12-15-2010.
  • Coyne JA, Orr HA. 2004. Speciation. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates.
  • Dahlheim ME, Schulman–Janiger A, Black N, Ternullo R, Ellifrit D, Balcomb KC III. 2008. Eastern temperate North Pacific offshore killer whales (Orcinus orca): occurrence, movements, and insights into feeding ecology. Marine Mammal Science 24: 719729.
  • Dahlheim ME, White PA. 2010. Ecological aspects of transient killer whales Orcinus orca as predators in southeastern Alaska. Wildlife Biology 16: 308322.
  • Danchin É, Wagner RH. 2008. Behavioural ecology. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Danchin É, Wagner RH. 2010. Inclusive heritability: combining genetic and non–genetic information to study animal behavior and culture. Oikos 119: 210218.
  • Deecke VB, Barrett-Lennard LG, Spong P, Ford JKB. 2010. The structure of stereotyped calls reflects kinship and social affiliation in resident killer whales (Orcinus orca). Naturwissenschaften 97: 513518.
  • Deecke VB, Ford JKB, Slater PJB. 2005. The vocal behaviour of mammal-eating killer whales: communicating with costly calls. Animal Behaviour 69: 395405.
  • Deecke VB, Ford JKB, Spong P. 2000. Dialect change in resident killer whales: implications for vocal learning and cultural transmission. Animal Behaviour 60: 629638.
  • Deecke VB, Nykänen M, Foote AD, Janik VM. 2011. Vocal behaviour and feeding ecology of killer whales (Orcinus orca) around Shetland, UK. Aquatic Biology 13: 7988.
  • Deecke VB, Slater PJB, Ford JKB. 2002. Selective habituation shapes acoustic predator recognition in harbour seals. Nature 420: 171173.
  • Dieckmann U, Doebeli M. 1999. On the origin of species by sympatric speciation. Nature 400: 354357.
  • Domenici P, Batty RS, Similä T, Ogam E. 2000. Killer whales (Orcinus orca) feeding on schooling herring (Clupea harengus) using underwater tail-slaps: kinematic analyses of field observations. Journal of Experimental Biology 203: 283294.
  • Duffield DA, Odell DK, McBain JF, Andrews B. 1995. Killer whale (Orcinus orca) reproduction at Sea World. Zoo Biology 14: 417430.
  • Ellis GM, Towers JR, Ford JKB. 2011. Northern resident killer whales of British Columbia: photo-identification catalogue and population status to 2010. Canadian Technical Report of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 2942: v + 71.
  • Filatova OA, Burdin AM, Hoyt E, Sato H. 2004. A catalogue of discrete calls of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) from the Avacha Gulf of Kamchatka Peninsula. Zoologichesky Zhurnal 83: 11691180.
  • Foote AD, Morin PA, Durban JW, Pitman RL, Wade P, Willerslev E, Gilbert MTP, da Fonseca RR. 2011a. Positive selection on the killer whale mitogenome. Biology Letters 7: 116118.
  • Foote AD, Morin PA, Durban JW, Willerslev E, Orlando L, Gilbert MTP. 2011b. Out of the Pacific and back again: insights into the matrilineal history of Pacific killer whale ecotypes. PLoS ONE 6: e24980.
  • Foote AD, Vilstrup JT, de Stephanis R, Verborgh P, Abel Nielsen SC, Deaville R, Kleivane L, Martin V, Miller PJO, Øien N, Pérez-Gil M, Rasmussen M, Reid RJ, Robertson KM, Rogan E, Similä T, Tejedor ML, Vester H, Víkingsson GA, Willerslev E, Gilbert MTP, Piertney SB. 2011c. Genetic differentiation among North Atlantic killer whale populations. Molecular Ecology 20: 629641.
  • Foote AD, Newton J, Piertney SB, Willerslev E, Gilbert MTP. 2009. Ecological, morphological and genetic divergence of sympatric North Atlantic killer whale populations. Molecular Ecology 18: 52075217.
  • Ford JKB. 1989. Acoustic behaviour of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off Vancouver Island, British Columbia. Canadian Journal of Zoology 67: 727745.
  • Ford JKB. 1991. Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal waters of British Columbia. Canadian Journal of Zoology 69: 14541483.
  • Ford JKB. 2009. Killer whale –Orcinus orca. In: Perrin WF, Würsig B, Thewissen JGM, eds. Encyclopedia of marine mammals, 2nd edn. Amsterdam: Academic Press, 650657.
  • Ford JKB, Ellis GM. 1999. Transients – mammal-hunting killer whales. Vancouver: UBC Press.
  • Ford JKB, Ellis GM. 2012. You are what you eat: foraging specializations and their influence on the social organization and behaviour of killer whales. In: Yamagiwa J, Karczmarski L, eds. Primates and cetaceans: field studies and conservation of complex mammalian societies. New York: Springer, in press.
  • Ford JKB, Ellis GM, Balcomb KC. 2000. Killer whales: the natural history and genealogy of Orcinus orca in British Columbia and Washington State, 2nd edn. Vancouver: UBC Press, and Seattle: University of Washington Press.
  • Ford JKB, Ellis GM, Barrett–Lennard LG, Morton AB, Palm RS, Balcomb KC III. 1998. Dietary specialization in two sympatric populations of killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal British Columbia and adjacent waters. Canadian Journal of Zoology 76: 14561571.
  • Ford JKB, Ellis GM, Matkin DR, Wetklo MH, Barrett-Lennard LG, Withler RE. 2011. Shark predation and tooth wear in a population of northeastern Pacific killer whales. Aquatic Biology 11: 213224.
  • Garland EC, Goldizen AW, Rekdahl ML, Constantine R, Garrigue C, Daeschler Hauser N, Poole MM, Robbins J, Noad MJ. 2011. Dynamic horizontal cultural transmission of humpback whale song at the ocean basin scale. Current Biology 21: 15.
  • Gavrilets S. 2004. Fitness landscapes and the origin of species. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
  • Gavrilets S, Vose A, Barluenga M, Salzburger W, Meyer A. 2007. Case studies and mathematical models of ecological speciation. 1. Cichlids in a crater lake. Molecular Ecology 16: 28932909.
  • Grant BR, Grant PR. 1996. Cultural inheritance of song and its role in the evolution of Darwin's finches. Evolution 50: 24712487.
  • Grant BR, Grant PR. 2010. Songs of Darwin's finches diverge when a new species enters the community. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 107: 2015620163.
  • Grant PR, Grant BR. 2009. The secondary contact phase of allopatric speciation in Darwin's finches. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 106: 2014120148.
  • Guinet C. 1991. Intentional stranding apprenticeship and social play in killer whales (Orcinus orca). Canadian Journal of Zoology 69: 27122716.
  • Guinet C, Bouvier J. 1995. Development of intentional stranding hunting techniques in killer whale (Orcinus orca) calves at Crozet Archipelago. Canadian Journal of Zoology 73: 2733.
  • Guinet C, Domenici P, de Stephanis R, Barrett-Lennard LG, Ford JKB, Verborgh P. 2007. Killer whale predation on bluefin tuna: exploring the hypothesis of the endurance-exhaustion technique. Marine Ecology Progress Series 347: 111119.
  • Hawks J, Wang ET, Cochran GM, Harpending HC, Moyzis RK. 2007. Recent acceleration of human adaptive evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104: 2075320758.
  • Heimlich SL, Boran JR. 1999. Social learning in cetaceans: hunting, hearing and hierarchies. In: Box HO, Gibson KR, eds. Mammalian social learning: comparative and ecological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 283307.
  • Hendry AP. 2009. Ecological speciation! Or the lack thereof? Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 66: 13831398.
  • Herman DP, Burrows DG, Wade PR, Durban JW, Matkin CO, LeDuc RG, Barrett-Lennard LG, Krahn MM. 2005. Feeding ecology of eastern North Pacific killer whales Orcinus orca from fatty acid, stable isotope, and organochlorine analyses of blubber biopsies. Marine Ecology Progress Series 302: 275291.
  • Hoelzel AR. 1991. Killer whale predation on marine mammals at Punta Norte, Argentina; food sharing, provisioning and foraging strategies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 29: 197204.
  • Hoelzel AR. 1998. Genetic structure of cetacean populations in sympatry, parapatry, and mixed assemblages: implications for conservation policy. Journal of Heredity 89: 451458.
  • Hoelzel AR, Dahlheim M, Stern SJ. 1998. Low genetic variation among killer whales (Orcinus orca) in the eastern North Pacific and genetic differentiation between foraging specialists. Journal of Heredity 89: 121128.
  • Hoelzel AR, Hey J, Dahlheim ME, Nicholson C, Burkanov V, Black N. 2007. Evolution of population structure in a highly social predator, the killer whale. Molecular Biology and Evolution 26: 14071415.
  • Hoelzel AR, Natoli A, Dahlheim ME, Olavarria C, Baird RW, Black NA. 2002. Low worldwide genetic diversity in the killer whale (Orcinus orca): implications for demographic history. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 269: 14671473.
  • Hoffman JI, Grant SM, Forcada J, Phillips CD. 2011. Bayesian inference of a historical bottleneck in a heavily exploited marine mammal. Molecular Ecology 20: 39894008.
  • Ivkovich T, Filatova OA, Burdin AM, Sato H, Hoyt E. 2010. The social organization of resident-type killer whales (Orcinus orca) in Avacha Gulf, Northwest Pacific, as revealed through association patterns and acoustic similarity. Mammalian Biology 75: 198210.
  • Janik VM. 2009. Acoustic communication in delphinids. Advances in the Study of Behavior 40: 123157.
  • Jones IM. 2006. A northeast Pacific offshore killer whale (Orcinus orca) feeding on a Pacific halibut (Hippoglossus stenolepis). Marine Mammal Science 22: 198200.
  • Krahn MM, Herman DP, Matkin CO, Durban JW, Barrett-Lennard L, Burrows DG, Dahlheim ME, Black N, LeDuc RG, Wade PR. 2007. Use of chemical tracers in assessing the diet and foraging regions of eastern North Pacific killer whales. Marine Environmental Research 63: 91114.
  • Krützen M, Mann J, Heithaus MR, Connor RC, Bejder L, Sherwin WB. 2005. Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 102: 89398943.
  • Laland KN, Galef BG, eds. 2009. The question of animal culture. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  • Laland KN, Janik VM. 2006. The animal cultures debate. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 21: 542547.
  • Laland KN, Odling-Smee J, Myles S. 2010. How culture shaped the human genome: bringing genetics and the human sciences together. Nature Reviews Genetics 11: 137148.
  • LeDuc RG, Robertson KM, Pitman RL. 2008. Mitochondrial sequence divergence among Antarctic killer whale ecotypes is consistent with multiple species. Biology Letters 4: 426429.
  • Lehmann L, Feldman MW. 2008. The co-evolution of culturally inherited altruistic helping and cultural transmission under random group formation. Theoretical Population Biology 4: 506516.
  • de León L, Bermingham E, Podos J, Hendry AP. 2010. Divergence with gene flow as facilitated by ecological differences: within-island variation in Darwin's finches. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 365: 10411052.
  • Li H, Durbin R. 2011. Inference of human population history from individual whole-genome sequences. Nature 475: 493496.
  • Lopez JC, Lopez D. 1985. Killer whales (Orcinus orca) of Patagonia, and their behavior of intentional stranding while hunting nearshore. Journal of Mammalogy 66: 181183.
  • Matkin C, Durban J. 2011. Killer whales in Alaskan waters. Journal of the American Cetacean Society 40: 2429.
  • Matkin CO, Barrett-Lennard LG, Yurk H, Ellifrit D, Trites AW. 2007. Ecotypic variation and predatory behavior among killer whales (Orcinus orca) off the eastern Aleutian Islands, Alaska. Fisheries Bulletin 105: 7487.
  • Matkin CO, Saulitis EL, Ellis GM, Olesiuk P, Rice SD. 2008. Ongoing population-level impacts on killer whales Orcinus orca following the ‘Exxon Valdez’ oil spill in Prince William Sound, Alaska. Marine Ecology Progress Series 356: 269281.
  • McGowen MR, Spaulding M, Gatesy J. 2009. Divergence date estimation and a comprehensive molecular tree of extant cetaceans. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 53: 891906.
  • Mesnick SL, Taylor BL, Le Duc RG, Treviño SE, O'Corry-Crowe GM, Dizon AE, Schlötterer C, Tiedemann R, Milinkovitch MC, Amos W, Whitehead H. 1999. Culture and genetic evolution in whales. Science 284: 2055a.
  • Miller PJO, Bain DE. 2000. Within-pod variation in the sound production of a pod of killer whales, Orcinus orca. Animal Behaviour 60: 617628.
  • Miller PJO, Shapiro AD, Deecke VB. 2010. The diving behaviour of mammal-eating killer whales (Orcinus orca): variations with ecological not physiological factors. Canadian Journal of Zoology 88: 11031112.
  • Moore SE, Francine JK, Bowles AE, Ford JKB. 1988. Analysis of calls of killer whales Orcinus orca from Iceland and Norway. Rit Fiskideildar 11: 225250.
  • Morin PA, Archer FI, Foote AD, Vilstrup J, Allen EE, Wade P, Durban J, Parsons K, Pitman R, Li L, Bouffard P, Abel Nielsen SC, Rasmussen M, Willerslev E, Gilbert MTP, Harkins T. 2010. Complete mitochondrial genome phylogeographic analysis of killer whales (Orcinus orca) indicates multiple species. Genome Research 20: 908916.
  • Moritz C. 1994. Defining ‘Evolutionary Significant Units’ for conservation. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 9: 373375.
  • Nolan CP, Liddle GM. 2000. Interactions between killer whales (Orcinus orca) and sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) with a longline fishing vessel. Marine Mammal Science 16: 658664.
  • Nosil P, Harmon LJ, Seehausen O. 2009. Ecological explanations for (incomplete) speciation. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 24: 145156.
  • Nosil P, Vines TH, Funk DJ. 2005. Perspective: reproductive isolation caused by natural selection against immigrants from divergent habitats. Evolution 59: 705719.
  • Nottestad L, Similä T. 2001. Killer whales attacking schooling fish: why force herring from deep water to the surface? Marine Mammal Science 17: 343352.
  • Nousek AE, Slater PJB, Wang C, Miller PJO. 2006. The influence of social affiliation on individual vocal signatures of northern resident killer whales (Orcinus orca). Biology Letters 2: 481484.
  • O'Brien SJ, Wildt DE, Goldman D, Merril CR, Bush M. 1983. The cheetah is depauperate in genetic variation. Science 221: 459462.
  • Osborne RW. 1986. A behavioral budget of Puget Sound killer whales. In: Kirkevold BC, Lockhard JS, eds. Behavioral biology of killer whales. New York: Alan R. Liss, Inc, 211249.
  • Pennisi E. 2008. Modernizing the modern synthesis. Science 321: 196197.
  • Perry GH, Dominy NJ, Claw KG, Lee AS, Fiegler H, Redon R, Werner J, Villanea FA, Mountain JL, Misra R, Carter NP, Lee C, Stone AC. 2007. Diet and the evolution of human amylase gene copy number variation. Nature Genetics 39: 12561260.
  • Pigliucci M. 2007. Do we need an extended evolutionary synthesis? Evolution 61: 27432749.
  • Pilot M, Dahlheim ME, Hoelzel AR. 2010. Social cohesion among kin, gene flow without dispersal and the evolution of population genetic structure in the killer whale (Orcinus orca). Journal of Evolutionary Biology 23: 2031.
  • Pitman RL. 2011. Antarctic killer whales: top of the food chain at the bottom of the world. Journal of the American Cetacean Society 40: 3945.
  • Pitman RL, Durban JW. 2012. Cooperative hunting behavior, prey selectivity and prey handling by pack ice killer whales (Orcinus orca), type B, in Antarctic Peninsula waters. Marine Mammal Science 28: 1636.
  • Pitman RL, Durban JW, Greenfelder M, Guinet C, Jorgensen M, Olson PA, Plana J, Tixier P, Towers JR. 2011. Observations of a distinctive morphotype of killer whale (Orcinus orca), type D, from subantarctic waters. Polar Biology 34: 303306.
  • Pitman RL, Ensor P. 2003. Three forms of killer whales (Orcinus orca) in Antarctic waters. Journal of Cetacean Research and Management 5: 131139.
  • Pitman RL, Perryman WL, LeRoi D, Eilers E. 2007. A dwarf form of killer whale in Antarctica. Journal of Mammalogy 88: 4348.
  • Price T. 2008. Speciation in birds. Greenwood Village: Robert & Company Publishers.
  • Rantala MJ, Marcinkowska UM. 2011. The role of sexual imprinting and the Westermarck effect in mate choice in humans. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65: 859873.
  • Reeves RR, Perrin WF, Taylor BL, Baker CS, Mesnick M, eds. 2004. Report of the workshop on shortcomings of cetacean taxonomy in relation to needs of conservation and management. April 30 – May 2, 2004, La Jolla, California: US Department of Commerce, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration, National Marine Fisheries Service, Southwest Fisheries Science Center, NOAA-TM-NMFS-SWFSC-363.
  • Rehn N, Filatova OA, Durban JW, Foote AD. 2011. Cross-cultural and cross-ecotype production of a killer whale ‘excitement’ call suggests universality. Naturwissenschaften 98: 16.
  • Rendell L, Whitehead H. 2001. Culture in whales and dolphins. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 309382.
  • Richerson PJ, Boyd R. 2005. Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Richlen MF, Thomas JA. 2008. Acoustic behavior of Antarctic killer whales (Orcinus orca) recorded near the ice edge of McMurdo Sound, Antarctica. Aquatic Mammals 34: 448457.
  • Riesch R, Deecke VB. 2011. Whistle communication in mammal-eating killer whales (Orcinus orca): further evidence for acoustic divergence between ecotypes. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65: 13771387.
  • Riesch R, Ford JKB, Thomsen F. 2006. Stability and group specificity of stereotyped whistles in resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off British Columbia. Animal Behaviour 71: 7991.
  • Riesch R, Ford JKB, Thomsen F. 2008. Whistle sequences in wild killer whales (Orcinus orca). Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 124: 18221829.
  • Rundle HD, Nosil P. 2005. Ecological speciation. Ecology Letters 8: 336352.
  • Ryan SJ. 2006. The role of culture in conservation planning for small or endangered populations. Conservation Biology 20: 13211324.
  • Sadedin S, Hollanders J, Panava M, Johannesson K, Gavrilets S. 2009. Case studies and mathematical models of ecological speciation. 3: ecotype formation in a Swedish snail. Molecular Ecology 18: 40064023.
  • Samarra FIP, Deecke VB, Vinding K, Rasmussen MH, Swift RJ, Miller PJO. 2010. Killer whales (Orcinus orca) produce ultrasonic whistles. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 128: EL205EL210.
  • Saulitis E, Matkin C, Barrett-Lennard L, Heise K, Ellis G. 2000. Foraging strategies of sympatric killer whale (Orcinus orca) populations in Prince William Sound, Alaska. Marine Mammal Science 16: 94109.
  • Schluter D. 2009. Evidence for ecological speciation and its alternative. Science 323: 737741.
  • Secchi ER, Vaske T Jr. 1998. Killer whale (Orcinus orca) sightings and depredation on tuna and swordfish longline catches in southern Brazil. Aquatic Mammals 24: 117122.
  • Seehausen O, Takimoto G, Roy D, Jokela J. 2008. Speciation reversal and biodiversity dynamics in changing environments. Molecular Ecology 17: 3044.
  • Similä T, Holst JC, Christensen I. 1996. Occurrence and diet of killer whales in northern Norway: seasonal patterns relative to the distribution and abundance of Norwegian spring–spawning herring. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 53: 769779.
  • Similä T, Ugarte F. 1993. Surface and underwater observations of cooperatively feeding killer whales in northern Norway. Canadian Journal of Zoology 71: 14941499.
  • Simon M, Hanson MB, Murrey L, Tougaard J, Ugarte F. 2009. From captivity to the wild and back: an attempt to release Keiko the killer whale. Marine Mammal Science 25: 693705.
  • Simon M, Wahlberg M, Ugarte F, Miller LA. 2005. Acoustic characteristics of underwater tail slaps used by Norwegian and Icelandic killer whales (Orcinus orca) to debilitate herring (Clupea harengus). Journal of Experimental Biology 208: 24592466.
  • Simoons FJ. 1978. The geographic hypothesis and lactose malabsorption. Digestestive Diseases 23: 963980.
  • Smith TG, Siniff DB, Reichle R, Stone S. 1981. Coordinated behavior of killer whales, Orcinus orca, hunting a crabeater seal, Lobodon carcinophagus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 59: 11851189.
  • Strager H. 1995. Pod-specific call repertoires and compound calls of killer whales, Orcinus orca Linnaeus, 1758, in the waters of northern Norway. Canadian Journal of Zoology 73: 10371047.
  • Tarasyan KK, Filatova OA, Burdin AM, Hoyt E, Sato H. 2005. Keys for the status of killer whales in Eastern Kamchatka, Russia: foraging ecology and acoustic behavior. Biosphere Conservation 6: 7383.
  • Taylor EB, Boughman JW, Groenenboom M, Sniatynski M, Schluter D, Gow JL. 2006. Speciation in reverse: morphological and genetic evidence of the collapse of a three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) species pair. Molecular Ecology 15: 343355.
  • Thomsen F, Franck D, Ford JKB. 2002. On the communicative significance of whistles in wild killer whales (Orcinus orca). Naturwissenschaften 89: 404407.
  • URL 1. Photo-identification of southern resident killer whales. Available at: http://www.whaleresearch.com/research.html Accessed on December 6, 2011.
  • URL 2. Population status of transient killer whales. Available at: http://www.pac.dfo-mpo.gc.ca/science/species-especes/cetacean-cetaces/projects-projets-eng.html Accessed on December 6, 2011.
  • URL 3. The captive killer whale Kalina. Available at: http://www.orca-spirit.co.uk/1210.html Accessed on November 23, 2011.
  • Via S. 2009. Natural selection in action during speciation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106: 99399946.
  • Visser IN. 1999. Benthic foraging on stingrays by killer whales (Orcinus orca) in New Zealand waters. Marine Mammal Science 15: 220227.
  • Visser IN. 2000. Killer whale (Orcinus orca) interactions with longline fisheries in New Zealand waters. Aquatic Mammals 26: 241252.
  • Visser IN, Smith TG, Bullock ID, Green GD, Carlsson OGL, Imberti S. 2008. Antarctic peninsula killer whales (Orcinus orca) hunt seals and a penguin on floating ice. Marine Mammal Science 24: 225234.
  • Weiß BM, Symonds H, Spong P, Ladich F. 2010. Call sharing across vocal clans of killer whales: evidence for vocal imitation? Marine Mammal Science 27: E1E13.
  • Weir BS, Cockerham CC. 1984. Estimating F-statistics for the analysis of population structure. Evolution 38: 13581370.
  • Whitehead H. 1998. Cultural selection and genetic diversity in matrilineal whales. Science 282: 17081711.
  • Whitehead H. 2010. Conserving and managing animals that learn socially and share cultures. Learning & Behavior 38: 329333.
  • Whiten A, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nishida T, Reynolds V, Sugiyama Y, Tutin CEG, Wrangham RW, Boesch C. 1999. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399: 682685.
  • Wolkers H, Corkeron PJ, Van Parijs SM, Similä T, van Bavel B. 2007. Accumulation and transfer of contaminants in killer whales (Orcinus orca) from Norway: indications for contaminant metabolism. Environmental Toxicology and Chemistry 26: 15821590.
  • Yurk H, Barrett-Lennard L, Ford JKB, Matkin CO. 2002. Cultural transmission within maternal lineages: vocal clans in resident killer whales in southern Alaska. Animal Behaviour 63: 11031119.