SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

Keywords:

  • adaptation;
  • correlational selection;
  • genetic constraints;
  • personality

Summary

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Conclusions
  8. Acknowledgements
  9. References
  • 1
    Animals often differ in suites of correlated behaviours, comparable with how humans differ in personality. Constraints on the architecture of behaviour have been invoked to explain why such ‘behavioural syndromes’ exist. From an adaptationist viewpoint, however, behavioural syndromes should evolve only in those populations where natural selection has favoured such trait covariance, and they should therefore exist only in particular types of population.
  • 2
    A comparative approach was used to examine this prediction of the adaptive hypothesis. We measured behavioural correlations in 12 different populations of three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) and assessed whether they indeed varied consistently according to the selective environment, where population was unit of analysis.
  • 3
    For a sample of fry from each population, we measured five different behaviours within the categories of (i) aggression (towards conspecifics); (ii) general activity; and (iii) exploration–avoidance (of novel foods, novel environments and altered environments).
  • 4
    We show that behavioural syndromes are not always the same in different types of stickleback population: the often-documented syndrome between aggressiveness, activity and exploratory behaviour existed only in large ponds where piscivorous predators were present. In small ponds where predators were absent, these behaviours were not (or only weakly) associated.
  • 5
    Our findings imply that population variation in behavioural syndromes does not result from stochastic evolutionary processes, but may result instead from adaptive evolution of behaviour favouring what should prove to be optimal trait combinations.

Introduction

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Conclusions
  8. Acknowledgements
  9. References

Many animal populations exhibit behavioural syndromes, individual differences in suites of correlated behaviours (Sih et al. 2004a), also referred to commonly as temperament (Réale et al. 2007) or animal personality (Gosling 2001). A behavioural syndrome is a characteristic of a population (Bell 2007) and exists whenever the same behaviour is correlated across different contexts, and/or when one type of behaviour is correlated with another of type behaviour. Examples of behavioural syndromes include positive correlations between aggressiveness in the context of territory defence and foraging in spiders (Riechert & Hedrick 1993), positive correlations between exploration and dispersal in birds (Dingemanse et al. 2003) and positive correlations between aggressiveness and boldness towards predators in fish (Huntingford 1976).

Behavioural syndromes may exist because of absolute constraints (sensu Hallgrímsson & Hall 2005) on the independent evolution of behaviours (Lande 1979; Loeschke 1987; Arnold 1992; Bell 2005) or because selection has favoured suites of correlated behaviours (Lande 1986; Price & Langen 1992; Dall, Houston & McNamara 2004; Dingemanse & Réale 2005; McElreath & Strimling 2006; Stamps 2007; Wolf et al. 2007). Despite rapidly accumulating evidence for the existence of behavioural syndromes in a wide variety of taxa (Réale et al. 2007), few empirical studies exist that have considered explicitly these evolutionary hypotheses (but see Riechert & Hedrick 1993; Bell 2005; Moretz et al. 2007; Bell & Sih, in press). Recently, however, Bell (2005) was able to reject the constraint hypothesis for the existence of a behavioural syndrome in three-spined stickleback Gasterosteus aculeatus by showing that the syndrome existed in a population with high numbers of predators, but not in a population with low numbers of predators. This finding implies that trait associations can, in principle, be altered in the course of evolution.

The population variation in behavioural syndromes of stickleback may exist either because selection favoured correlated behaviours only in particular types of selective environment (hereafter called the adaptive hypothesis), as hypothesized by Bell (2005), or because of stochastic processes such as mutation, drift, founder effects or gene flow (Lande 1992; Armbruster & Schwaegerle 1996; Whitlock, Phillips & Fowler 2002). Hence, rejection of the constraint hypothesis does not necessarily equate to acceptance of the adaptive hypothesis (contra Bell 2005). Of these explanations, fortunately, the adaptive hypothesis is the only one that generally predicts an association between a behavioural correlation and the environmental characteristics of a population. This unique prediction can be evaluated by means of a comparison, using replicates of different types of population. Therefore, this study is aimed specifically at evaluating whether behavioural syndromes exist only in particular types of population, where population is the unit of analysis.

We examined whether behavioural traits were indeed structured more tightly into syndromes in populations that live sympatrically with predators (hereafter called predator-sympatric) compared with populations where predators were absent (hereafter called predator-naïve). For stickleback fry from each of six predator-sympatric and six predator-naive populations, we measured five behaviours indicative of (i) intraspecific aggressiveness; (ii) general activity; and (iii) exploration–avoidance (of novel foods, novel environments, and altered environments). We then analysed whether predation regime explained between-population variation in the within-population correlations between these behaviours.

Methods

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Conclusions
  8. Acknowledgements
  9. References

study populations

The 12 study populations were all ponds on the island of Anglesey, North Wales, UK, about 20–100 years old and situated between 1·8 and 28·2 km apart. In six ponds, piscivorous predators (perch Perca perca, pike Esox lucius and/or rainbow trout Oncorhynchus mykiss) had been introduced (directly after the ponds were created) and maintained at high densities, and in a further six ponds piscivorous predators had not been introduced nor observed during any ecological surveys (Countryside Council for Wales, personal communication). The presence of piscivorous predators was correlated highly with pond size: piscivorous predators were present in the six largest ponds, but were absent in the smallest six ponds. Therefore, we do not attempt to differentiate between the effects of pond size and predation regime. We nevertheless consider predators to be the most likely cause of any possible selection pressure on behavioural syndromes (see Introduction and Discussion), and therefore use predator presence/absence as a label to distinguish small ponds lacking piscivorous predators from large ponds containing such predators.

Gene flow between these 12 ponds is largely restricted, and presence of piscivorous predators is independent of ancestry, as shown by phylogenetic analyses (Dingemanse et al., unpublished). Any evolutionary change in response to the presence of piscivorous predators will have been independent of ancestry, and population can therefore be treated as the independent unit of analysis.

experimental protocol

Stickleback fry were collected in six batches over a period of 6 successive weeks in July/August 2003, several populations being sampled within each batch. Fish were transported to the laboratory, kept overnight in holding tanks, and then subjected to a series of experiments for a period of 12 days (see Table 1). The day following capture (day 1), size (standard length) of each fish was measured. Each individual was then assigned to one of three size categories within batch and population: (i) ‘small’ (lower quartile); (ii) ‘medium’ (middle two quartiles combined); and (iii) ‘large’ (upper quartile). Only ‘large’ fish were used as experimental subjects (n = 7 per population; mean ± SE size = 20·81 ± 0·16 mm), while ‘small’ fish were used as stimuli in the aggression test described below (n = 7 per population; size = 17·18 ± 0·20 mm). This allowed easy identification of the subject fish vs. the intruder during interactions, and avoided issues connected with marking individuals. This selection procedure based on size did not bias the data, as size did not correlate with any of the assayed behaviours (see Results). Notably, the lack of correlation between standard length and behaviour has been confirmed in a recent breeding experiment (Dingemanse et al. in preparation).

Table 1.  Time-table of experimental procedures (see Methods for more detail). The main behavioural tests are highlighted in bold type
DayAction/behavioural test type
0Capture and transfer to laboratory
1Size measurements, individual housing (09.00–10.00 h)
1–4Settling period, feeding training (16.00 h)
5Transfer to experimental room (09.00–10.00 h)
5–7Settling period, feeding training (16.00 h)
81. Activity and aggression test (09.00–12.00 h)
2. Activity and familiar food feeding (16.00–18.00 h)
91. Activity and novel environment test (09.00–15.30 h)
2. Activity and familiar food feeding (16.00–18.00 h)
101. Activity and control for predator test (09.00–12.00 h)
2. Activity and novel food test (16.00–18.00 h)
111. Activity and predator test (09.00–12.00 h)
2. Activity and familiar food feeding (16.00– 18.00 h)
12Release into the wild

Subjects were housed in isolation in tanks 30 × 20 × 22 cm (L × W × H), containing 10 cm depth of water, a filter with aeration and a clump of artificial weed attached to a stone (used as a refuge). Subjects were allowed to settle for 4 days (days 1–4), and were trained to feed on a familiar food item (one frozen bloodworm), provided daily at 16·00 h from a fixed location (see Fig. 1a). They were then transferred, in water, to individual tanks in an experimental room (day 5) that were lined internally with opaque white polythene to minimize external influences during the experiments. Each tank was positioned on top of a transparent sheet containing 126 uniquely numbered 2 × 2 cm squares to facilitate recording of subject location, and was placed 3 cm from an experimental tank (half the size of the home tank) within a large water-bath, to minimize potential visual interference created by air–glass–water interfaces (Fig. 1a). Subjects settled and were fed familiar food items for another three days (days 5–7), and were then subjected to a range of behavioural tests during the following 4 days (days 8–11). Fish were maintained at 19 ± 1 °C throughout, under a 15 h light/9 h dark illumination cycle.

image

Figure 1. Diagram of the experimental set-up. (a) Arrangement of the subject tank (‘s’) and experimental tank (‘p’) contained within a larger water bath (for dimensions see text). The subject tank contained a refuge (illustrated), and both tanks were lined internally on three sides with opaque white polythene. The asterisk (*) denotes the position where the intruder fish was introduced (aggression test) and where food was provided (during familiar food feeding and novel food test). The dotted line inside the subject tank indicates position of the opaque barrier that was removed at onset of the control for the predator test; the dotted line outside the subject tank shows position of same removable barrier in the 15-min period prior to onset of the predator test (for further details see Methods). (b) Set-up of the novel environment tank, showing the area in which the subject was confined directly after capture from its home tank (upper left corner), and the arrangement of stones during the subsequent test.

Download figure to PowerPoint

behavioural tests

We measured three categories of behaviour (following ecological terminology outlined by Réale et al. 2007) that have often been found to constitute a syndrome, namely: aggressiveness (an individual's agonistic reaction towards a conspecific intruder); activity (the general level of activity in a non-novel and non-risky environment) and exploration–avoidance (an individual's reaction to a new situation). A different behavioural trial was carried out daily on each fish from day 8 to day 11. Each trial started by first scoring general activity of the subject in its own tank, followed by either an experimental test or feeding of the subject (see Table 1). Various behavioural measures were taken during each trial (listed in Table 2). Each trial was run by one of three observers, with trials being randomized between observers within batches. A randomized block design (Sokal & Rohlf 1995) was used for trials with respect to time of day and population, and all individuals received the tests in the same order to facilitate comparison between individuals.

Table 2.  Results of principal component analyses on behavioural measures recorded, for each of five behavioural tests; (a) activity in a familiar environment; (b) intraspecific aggressiveness; and exploration of (c) a novel environment; (d) a novel food (fish flake); and (e) an altered environment (visual contact with predator or new adjacent place). Loadings, eigenvalues and explained variance are given for the emerging axes. Sample sizes are (a) n = 8 observations for each of 84 individuals (total n = 672); (b–d) n = 1 observation for each of 84 individuals (total n = 84); (e) n = 2 observations for each of 84 individuals (total n = 168)
(a) Locomotor activityComponent 1 (A1) 
 Behaviour
   No. of unique squares visited0·98 
   Distance travelled0·96 
   Number of square changes0·99 
 Eigenvalue2·85 
 % variance explained95·08 
(b) Aggression testComponent 1 (B1) 
 Behaviour
   Mean minimal distance from intruder–0·71 
   Total number of bites0·90 
   Prop. of 10-s interval with biting0·94 
 Eigenvalue2·19 
 % variance explained73·02 
(c) Novel environment testComponent 1 (C1) 
 Behaviour
   No. of unique squares visited0·96 
   Distance travelled0·94 
   Number of square changes0·99 
 Eigenvalue2·78 
 % variance explained92·70 
(d) Novel food testComponent 1 (D1) 
 Behaviour
   Distance from food at start0·39 
   Latency to first inspection0·87 
   Latency to first bite0·87 
   Minimum distance from food0·89 
 Eigenvalue2·45 
 % variance explained61·30 
(e) Altered environment test (predator test and control)Component 1 (E1)Component 2 (E2)
 Number of square changes0·94–0·09
   Mean angle of orientation–0·040·97
   SD angle of orientation0·70–0·54
   Minimum distance from glass–0·760·12
   No. of inspections0·93–0·04
 Eigenvalue2·831·26
 % variance explained56·6525·28

activity

Activity was measured in a non-risky and non-novel environment for eight different trials (two per day, days 8–11; Table 1), by observing movements of each subject within its home tank for a 60-s period. Position of the subject was noted every 5 s using point-time sampling.

aggressiveness

Aggressiveness was measured as the subject's agonistic response towards a smaller stimulus intruder fish, introduced into the subject's home tank (day 8; Table 1; see location in Fig. 1a) for a period of 120 s, and then removed. This provides a more realistic stimulus than an intruder confined behind glass, in that the subject receives appropriate sensory feedback from both its own and the opponent's actions. Importantly, a glass container might have been perceived as a novel object, thus potentially confounding responses to the object with those to the conspecific in the behavioural measure obtained. While subject behaviour in both types of live interaction may depend in part upon that of the stimulus fish, in our case each stimulus fish was used only once to avoid pseudoreplication (Hurlbert 1984) – hence, any variation in the behaviour of different intruders can simply be regarded as adding (unbiased) noise.

Stimuli fish were assigned to subjects according to their respective rank positions within their own size category (i.e. larger subjects paired with the larger of the small stimuli fish). All subjects acted aggressively towards the intruder, as manifested by chasing and attempted biting (rather than attempting to maintain proximity, i.e. shoaling), whereas the stimulus fish was generally submissive. The minimal distance between subject and stimulus and the number of times the subject attempted to bite the latter within each 10-s interval were measured.

exploration–avoidance of a novel environment

On day 9 (Table 1), subjects were captured from their home tanks, transferred to a confined area (4 × 6 cm, six squares) in the upper-left corner of a similar-sized tank (Fig. 1b) and left to acclimatize for a period of 300 s. The barrier was then removed, allowing the subject to explore the remainder of this novel environment for a further 300 s. The tank contained five large stones, each covering an area of approximately 4 × 5 cm (see Fig. 1b). The tank was filled with only 5 cm of water to prevent the subject from swimming above the stones: information about the environment could thus only be gained by moving around the stones. Following release from the confined area, position of the subject was noted every 5 s using point-time sampling.

exploration–avoidance of a novel food

On day 10 (Table 1) each fish received a piece of commercial fish flake (about 0·5 × 0·5 cm) instead of the food it was trained to receive during the preceding 9 days. The flake constituted a novel food type, and accordingly the fish showed inspection behaviour (as defined by Huntingford 1976) prior to biting the flake. The subject's latency until first inspection and latency until first bite were recorded, and the position of both the subject and the flake noted every 10 s for 2 min using point-time sampling.

exploration–avoidance of an altered environment

The initial aim of the behavioural test described here was to measure shyness–boldness (an individual's reaction to any risky, but not new, situation; Réale et al. 2007). This behaviour was measured by visually exposing the subject to a predator for 5 min. On day 11 (see Table 1), a live perch P. fluviatilis was introduced into the tank, the end of which was placed 5 cm from the end of the subject's tank (Fig. 1a). Fifteen minutes later, the opaque barrier visually isolating the two tanks was removed to reveal the predator. After barrier removal, position and angle of the subject's body (in steps of 45°) were noted every 5 s using point-time sampling. We further noted the total number of inspection bouts. Inspection bout was defined as starting when the subject faced the experimental tank while swimming towards the stimulus tank, and terminated when the subject turned away from the stimulus (Walling et al. 2004). Seventy individual perch (mean ± SE total length = 13·335 ± 0·162 cm, n = 45 measured) were used as stimuli.

To ascertain whether the elicited behaviour reflected boldness towards the predator, rather than exploration of the altered environment of the tank, we first ran the testing procedure without the predator. The day preceding the predator test (day 10, Table 1), the subject was shown an empty tank. This arrangement was left in place for 24 h to familiarize the subject with its altered environment. The barrier was replaced only 15 min prior to the onset of the predator trial, and then raised to commence the trial. Analyses revealed a relatively high repeatability (for a behavioural trait; Stirling, Réale & Roff 2002) at the individual level of the subject's behaviour (0·455; see Results; Table 3), with the effect of trial type being negligible (experimental day in Table 3; see Results). Behaviour in both trials therefore appeared to measure primarily how subjects reacted to an alteration of their physical environment (i.e. exploration, not boldness). We therefore regarded the predator and control trials as repeated measures of the same behaviour, hereafter referred to as exploration of an altered environment.

Table 3.  Sources of variation in activity in a familiar environment (A1 in Table 2a), intraspecific aggressiveness (B1 in Table 2b), and exploration of a novel environment (C1 in Table 2c), a novel food (D1 in Table 2d) and an altered environment (E1 in Table 2e). The results are from GLMMs with normal errors, with population (all tests) and individual nested within population (activity and altered environment tests) entered as random effects, using backward elimination of non-significant fixed effects. Population and individual repeatabilities (calculated after Rasbash et al. 2004) are given in parentheses; significant P-values are highlighted in bold type. For sample sizes see Table 2
Behaviour variableActivityAggressivenessExploration of:
Novel environmentNovel foodAltered environment
inline imagePinline imagePinline imagePinline imagePinline imageP
  • *

    Combined effect of all fixed effects kept in final model; NA: all fixed effects were non-significant.

  • §

    §Final model is significant after sequential Bonferroni adjustment of error rates.

Fixed effects
 Final model*8·96620·011§NANA3·82710·05011·96020·002§10·61820·004§
 Week5·13410·0231·40810·2353·82710·0509·24310·0025·91410·015
 Observer1·66420·4353·73320·1553·84610·1461·88520·3901·09020·580
 Time of day0·09010·7640·20410·6520·42810·5126·95310·0080·38010·538
 Size (population level)0·05610·8130·04010·8411·56210·4530·25010·6170·10110·751
 Size (individual level)0·00011·0000·00310·9560·27110·6020·05010·8230·01610·899
 Experimental day3·83310·0504·70410·030
 Trial1·07810·299
Random effects
 Population1·56910·2100·07010·7913·61810·0572·83710·0921·45310·228
 (repeatability)(0·155) (0·018) (0·336) (0·244) (0·187) 
 Individual26·2311< 0·000112·55210·0003
 (repeatability)(0·424)(0·455)  

ethical note

Behavioural testing was conducted in accordance with UK regulations governing care and use of laboratory animals and had prior approval of the local Home Office. During the aggression tests, the small stimulus fish was always able to evade the subject easily, and no signs of injury were observed during this brief experience. Upon the completion of all tests within each batch, stickleback were released into a private pond in north-west Wales (day 12; Table 1), and perch were released back into ponds of the fishery from which they were obtained.

statistical analyses

Extraction of behavioural axes

Principal component analysis (PCA) followed by varimax rotation (Tabachnick & Fidell 2001) was used to summarize the behavioural measures for each type of test (Table 2). Individuals were usually represented once in these analyses, but eight times for activity tests and twice for altered environment tests. The resulting PCA scores per individual were then used in further analyses.

We included repeated measures of the same individual within these PCAs for the activity and altered environment tests, thereby violating the assumption of independent observations (Tabachnick & Fidell 2001). Therefore, to confirm the validity of these analyses, we also ran PCAs for each trail separately (activity: eight PCAs; altered environment: two PCAs), which produced reassuringly very similar components (results not shown), thereby justifying combining these data from multiple trials in one analysis.

estimation of individual phenotypes

Generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) were used to: (i) estimate effects of fixed effects that could bias the data (in our case, seasonal or time of day effects; Table 3); and (ii) subtract phenotypic values at the level of individual (so-called best linear unbiased predictors – BLUPS; Henderson 1975) while controlling for significant fixed effects. Population (n = 12) was fitted as a random effect; individual (nested within population; n = 84) was also fitted as a random effect for those tests where repeated measures of the same individual were taken (data points per individual, n = 8 in activity tests, and n = 2 in altered environment tests). The following continuous (co) and categorical (ca) fixed effects were fitted in the initial model (Table 3): week (co; to model effects of season), time of day (co), observers (c), mean population value of body size (‘size population level’; co), and individual body size expressed as deviations from the population mean (‘size individual level’; co). For activity, trial type (feeding vs. non-feeding trials; c) and experimental day (co) were also fitted as fixed effects to evaluate whether activity differed between the two trial periods and whether activity changed over days, respectively. Similarly, in the case of the altered environment test, experimental day (c) was fitted as a fixed effect to test for differences between the control and the predator trials. Backward elimination of non-significant fixed effects (P > 0·05) was used as a model selection criterion (Crawley 1993). To check whether lack of significance of certain fixed effects (see Results; Table 3) was caused by over-parameterization of the models, we also re-ran each model including only one fixed effect at the time. These analyses gave the same outcomes (results not shown), implying that over-parameterization was not an issue here. We controlled statistically for significant fixed effects (see above) only in cases where the final model was significant after Bonferroni adjustment of experimental error rates (see below). We used the Wald statistic to evaluate statistical significance. BLUPS were expressed in SD units for further analyses.

sources of variation in behavioural syndromes

For each population, we calculated Spearman's rank correlations between the BLUPS (see above) of each of the five behaviours. One-way analyses of variance (anovas) were then used to assess whether behavioural correlations differed between predator-sympatric vs. predator-naïve populations, where population was unit of analysis. Correlation coefficients were Fisher's Z transformed prior to analysis (Zar 1999). Mann–Whitney U-tests were used to evaluate whether variances differed between the two types of population.

Sequential Bonferroni adjustment of experimental error rates was applied to account for multiple testing (Sokal & Rohlf 1995) in cases where ‘families of tests’ were conducted to evaluate the same hypothesis (Chandler 1995). Because such type I error control is over-conservative and may lead to type II errors (Moran 2003), we also present an overall test of each of the main hypotheses (Table 4), by counting the number of significant results and calculating the probability of finding that particular number by chance alone (where the value of P was calculated using a Bernoulli process, following Moran 2003).

Table 4.  Sources of variation in Spearman's rank correlations (r) between five behavioural traits. For each pair of behaviours, we present the average within-population r ± SE, separately for predator-naïve populations (n = 6) and predator-sympatric populations (n = 6), and test whether distributions of within-population correlations differ from zero. We further test (using one-way anovas) whether population type explains variation in r (n = 12). Prior to analyses, correlations were transformed using Fisher's Z transformation for correlation coefficients (Zar 1999). Population is the unit of analysis, with significant P-values highlighted in bold type
Behaviour pairsSmall ponds without predatorsLarge ponds with predatorsPopulation type
r ± SEt5Pr ± SEt5PF1,10Pr2
  • *

    Significant after sequential Bonferroni correction.

  • §

    Probability of finding this number of statistically significant tests due to chance alone; value of P is calculated using a Bernoulli process (following Moran 2003).

Activity–aggressiveness–0·089 ± 0·106–0·8280·4450·411 ± 0·0566·4560·001*17·0360·002*0·593
Activity–novel environment0·244 ± 0·1151·9120·1140·754 ± 0·0518·809< 0·001*15·6510·003*0·571
Activity–novel food–0·363 ± 0·120–2·4370·059–0·143 ± 0·103–1·3790·2262·0010·1880·083
Activity–altered environment0·286 ± 0·1711·7230·1460·667 ± 0·0508·061< 0·001*4·5780·0580·245
Aggressiveness–novel environment–0·202 ± 0·202–1·0530·3400·436 ± 0·1362·7880·0396·6350·0280·339
Aggressiveness–novel food0·048 ± 0·1830·3680·7280·232 ± 0·4351·5520·1810·4050·5390·039
Aggressiveness–altered environment–0·101 ± 0·096−1·0640·3360·435 ± 0·1352·6590·0458·1690·0170·395
Novel environment–novel food–0·470 ± 0·135–2·9040·0340·090 ± 0·1150·7990·4618·4710·0160·404
Novel environment–altered environment0·196 ± 0·0812·3520·0650·623 ± 0·1154·4250·007*9·1400·0130·425
Novel food–altered environment–0·202 ± 0·145–1·3030·249–0·036 ± 0·193–0·2230·8320·5250·4860·050
Number of significant tests  1 of 10  6 of 10 6 of 10 
P§  0·315  < 0·001 < 0·001 

The data were analysed using MLwiN version 2·0 (mixed model analyses; Rasbash et al. 2004) and SPSS version 13·0 (all other analyses; SPSS Inc. 1988). All tests were two-tailed and a critical value of P = 0·05 was applied throughout.

Results

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Conclusions
  8. Acknowledgements
  9. References

axes of behaviour

In each of the five PCAs, the first principal component described the behaviour of interest (A1–E1 in Table 2). For all subsequent analyses, we therefore used the PCA scores derived from these first components.

A1 described ‘activity in a familiar environment’, with high positive loadings for all measures of activity.

B1 described ‘aggressiveness’, with positive loadings for both the number of attempted bites and the proportion of intervals with attempted biting, and negative loadings for the distance between the subject and the stimulus fish. Aggressive subjects actively chased the intruder.

C1 described activity in the novel tank, with high positive loadings for all measures of activity. Subjects actively inspected the environment, whereas they did not show such behaviour during the activity trials in their home tank. While swimming through the novel tank, the number of unique squares visited per minute was over 1·9 times higher than during the activity trial in the home tank prior to onset of the test (mean ± SE = 4·860 ± 2·400 vs. 2·496 ± 0·228; paired t-test: t83 = −8·874, P < 0·0001, n = 84 individuals). Activity in the familiar vs. the novel environment was not simply a repeated measure of the same trait, because the behaviours were not correlated tightly in all populations (see Results, section ‘Sources of variation in behavioural syndromes’). We therefore considered the behaviour in the novel tank as a measure of ‘exploration of novel environments’ not activity, following Réale et al. (2007).

D1 described ‘avoidance of novel food’, with high positive loadings for time until first approach or biting, and high positive loadings for the subject's minimum distance from the food during the test. D1 did not simply reflect differences in hunger levels, because the percentage of fish biting the novel food within 30 s was relatively low (63·1%) compared with the percentage biting the familiar food on the preceding day (98·8%; logistic regression,inline image = 34·743, P < 0·0001, n = 84 individuals) and the day after the novel food trial (97·6%; inline image = 31·714, P < 0·0001, n = 84 individuals). D1 was multiplied by −­1, so that in further analyses it indicated exploration – rather than avoidance – of the novel food.

E1 described ‘exploration of the altered environment’, with high positive loadings for variables describing movement, number of inspections and variation in the angle of orientation: inspecting fish moved towards the stimulus, then turned around, returned to cover and initiated a new inspection, whereas non-inspecting fish remained still and did not come near the glass interface. A second component (E2) described ‘absence of attention to the stimulus’, with high positive loadings for the mean angle of orientation.

sources of variation in single behaviours

None of the behavioural traits were biased due to observer effects or subject size (Table 3). Two fixed effects that could potentially bias the data did explain significant variation in some of the behaviours (Table 3). First, activity (A1), exploration of novel food (D1) and exploration of altered environments (E1) decreased with week within populations. Secondly, exploration of novel food increased with time of day. To avoid potential bias, these effects were controlled for statistically in all further analyses (see Methods).

Although significant, exploration of altered environments was only 0·288 ± 0·105 SD units higher during the predator test than during the control test (experimental day in Table 3). Subjects showed only slightly more inspection behaviour when confronted with the predator than with an empty tank. This effect of test type did not differ between populations (GLMM with random slopes; interaction experimental day × population; variance in within-population slopes ± SE = 0·030 ± 0·068, inline image = 0·193, P = 0·660, n = 2 observations for each of seven individuals within each of 12 populations), implying that predators did not elicit stronger additional responses from subjects that originated from predator-sympatric populations. Individuals were repeatable over the two trials (Table 3) whereas trial type explained only 5·3% of the within-individual variance. These two trials were therefore regarded as repeated measures of the same exploration behaviour of altered environments.

sources of variation in behavioural syndromes

Predator-sympatric populations were characterized predictably by the presence of a behavioural syndrome: the average correlation was positive and significant for six of 10 behaviour pairs (Table 4). Activity, aggressiveness, exploration of novel environments and exploration of altered environments were all correlated within this type of pond: animals that were relatively aggressive were also relatively active in a familiar environment and were relatively explorative in novel and altered environments, compared to less aggressive animals. However, for two of these six cases, the average phenotypic correlation was not significant after accounting for multiple testing (Table 4), implying that these particular findings should be considered less than definitive. Nevertheless, the probability of finding six of 10 significant tests was significantly greater than expected by chance (Table 4), implying that the existence of a behavioural syndrome was supported with high confidence. Exploration of novel foods was consistently not part of the behavioural syndrome (Table 4; Fig. 2).

image

Figure 2. Behavioural syndromes for two types of populations: (a) predator-naïve (n = 6); and (b) predator-sympatric (n = 6). For each pair of behaviours, we present the average within-population r ± SE. Solid lines indicate significant correlations (P < 0·05; for statistical analyses see Table 4).

Download figure to PowerPoint

Predator-naïve populations were characterized predictably by the absence of a behavioural syndrome (Table 4; Fig. 2). The average correlation was significant for only one of 10 behaviour pairs: exploration of novel environments and novel foods were correlated negatively. This single significant correlation was, notably, not significant after accounting for multiple testing (Table 4). Moreover, the probability of finding one of 10 significant tests by chance was high (Table 4), suggesting that this result was likely to represent a type I error.

Consistent with the findings outlined above, behavioural correlations differed predictably between the two types of population: for six of 10 behaviour pairs, the average correlation was significantly more positive for predator-sympatric populations (Table 4). However, for four of these six cases, the average phenotypic correlation was not significant after accounting for multiple testing (Table 4), implying that these particular findings should be considered less than definitive. Nevertheless, the probability of finding this number of significant tests was significantly greater than expected by chance (Table 4), implying that the existence of predictable population differences in behavioural correlations was supported with high confidence.

The lack of correlation between behaviours in predator-naïve populations may have been caused either by a lack of between-individual variation in single behaviours (i.e. all individuals were of the same behavioural type), or by a true lack of association between behaviours (i.e. individuals did differ in single behaviours but at the same time different behaviours were simply not associated; see Coleman & Wilson 1998). We therefore tested for each trait whether the amount of between-individual variation differed between the two types of population, which was not the case (Table 5). Moreover, for two traits where we had multiple records per individual, significant and comparable individual repeatabilities were found within both types of population. Average within-population repeatability of activity in the familiar environment was 0·426 (inline image = 13·166, P < 0·0001; hierarchical GLMM with observation, individual and population as random effects) for predator-naïve ponds, and 0·417 (inline image = 15·186, P < 0·0001) for predator-sympatric ponds (n = 8 observations for each of seven individuals within each of six populations (total n = 336) in both cases); repeatability of exploration of altered environments was 0·552 (inline image = 8·521, P = 0·003) for predator-naïve ponds, and 0·339 (inline image = 3·832, P = 0·050) for predator-sympatric ponds (n = 2 observations for each of seven individuals within each of six populations (total n = 84) in both cases). The general lack of correlation between traits in predator-naïve populations was therefore not caused by a lack of individual variation. In contrast, it appears to be a genuine lack of association between behaviours in this particular environment.

Table 5.  Between-individual variances for five behavioural traits for each of 12 populations; we tested whether variances differed between predator-sympatric vs. predator-naïve populations
BehaviourActivityAggressivenessExploration of:
PopulationNovel environmentNovel foodAltered environment
Predator-naïve
 Bengyl 3·211 0·589 0·760 0·313 1·308
 Bryngwyn Bach 0·205 0·674 0·278 0·977 0·774
 Cae Mawr 1·018 0·858 0·584 0·110 0·658
 Cors Bordeilio 0·551 0·751 0·777 1·995 0·829
 Llyn Grigyll 0·091 0·698 0·152 0·578 0·610
 Rhos-y-Gad 0·067 1·206 0·439 0·867 0·753
Predator-sympatric
 Llyn Alaw 0·511 2·409 1·251 1·789 0·786
 Llyn Coron 1·018 0·782 0·954 0·724 0·630
 Llyn Llywenan 0·200 0·885 1·343 1·391 0·419
 Llyn Pen-y-parc 1·165 1·087 0·234 1·210 0·978
 Llyn Traffwll 1·123 0·639 0·956 1·308 0·510
 Tacan 0·631 1·065 0·147 0·590 0·573
Mann–Whitney U121011 1010
P 0·337 0·200 0·262 0·200 0·200

Discussion

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Conclusions
  8. Acknowledgements
  9. References

population variation in behavioural syndromes

The adaptive hypothesis for the existence of behavioural syndromes predicts that they should evolve only in populations where they are favoured by natural selection. If so, then population characteristics should explain between-population variation in behavioural correlations that constitute syndromes. The results presented here confirm this prediction: behavioural correlations between aggressiveness, activity in the familiar environment and exploration of novel and altered environments were significantly tighter, or tended to be so, in predator-sympatric populations, when compared with predator-naïve populations (Table 4; Fig. 2). These findings imply that population variation in behavioural syndromes does not simply result from stochastic evolutionary processes (random effects of mutation, drift or founder effects). Our findings thus show that behavioural syndromes within a species are not ubiquitous, and that when they do exist they may well result from natural selection favouring optimal trait combinations.

The effect of population type (Table 4; Fig. 2) was significant not because the two types of populations differed in amount of phenotypic variation in behavioural traits: phenotypic variances (Table 5) and individual repeatabilities were similar between the two types of populations. Thus, the lack of correlations between behaviours in predator-naïve populations (Fig. 2a) was not because all individuals had the same ‘behavioural type’ (sensu Sih, Bell & Johnson 2004b): individuals did differ in their behaviour but the behavioural traits completely lacked, or showed very weak, covariance.

environmental correlates of behavioural syndromes

Bell (2005) hypothesized that predators might shape behavioural syndromes in stickleback: aggressiveness, boldness and exploration were correlated tightly in one population with presumed high predation risk, whereas those traits were not correlated in another population with presumed low predation risk. Due to a lack of replication, she was unable to evaluate statistically the occurrence of a correlation between predation regime and behavioural syndromes. Using replicates of different population types, our study confirms that behavioural syndromes are indeed correlated with the presence of piscivorous predators.

It is important to note, however, that piscivorous predators are most likely to occur only in certain types of pond habitats and other correlated factors (e.g. competitive regimes, pond sizes, temperature regimes, habitat heterogeneity) may instead be the cause of between population variation in behavioural syndromes. We have therefore recently conducted an experiment to test whether piscivorous predators do tighten behavioural correlations (Dingemanse et al. in preparation). Experience with predators during ontogeny tightened the genetic correlation between activity and exploration of novel environments in stickleback fry originating from one of the predator-naïve populations. Therefore, we feel confident that piscivorous predators are the probable causal affect leading to the expression of behavioural correlations, as suggested by the results of the present study.

The obvious next step is to confirm the adaptive nature of behavioural syndromes in predator-sympatric populations, by testing experimentally whether predators do indeed induce correlational selection gradients (Dingemanse & Réale 2005), and the behavioural and selective mechanisms by which this effect comes about. Presence of predators may, for instance, induce spatial variation in predation risk within ponds and consequently favour the evolution of alternative behavioural strategies. As an effective antipredator strategy individuals might, for instance, either adopt a solitary strategy and monopolize a patch of habitat that is relatively poor foraging but safe, or adopt a shoaling strategy and roam patches of habitat that are relatively dangerous but likely to contain more food (Krause & Ruxton 2002). Such alternative strategies would have associated costs and benefits that are both density and frequency-dependent (Wilson et al. 1994). They might also have different optimal levels of both aggressiveness and exploratory behaviour, resulting in a correlational selection gradient within predator-sympatric populations. Individuals adopting a solitary strategy might have to be relatively aggressive, because competition for safe patches would be intense. Individuals adopting a shoaling strategy, in contrast, might have to be relatively non-aggressive, because high levels of fighting might offset any benefits of shoaling. Individuals adopting a solitary strategy might also have to be relatively explorative compared with individuals adopting a shoaling strategy, in order to offset the lack of socially acquired information (Sih 1997). In freshwater fish, individuals with solitary tendencies are indeed relatively explorative and relatively aggressive (e.g. Ward et al. 2004). Importantly, predatory fish have recently been shown to induce correlational selection gradients in artificial laboratory environments (Bell & Sih, in press), but we do not yet know whether natural selection also favours the evolution of behavioural correlation in the wild. However, it is important to note here that various other scenarios are equally conceivable (Sinervo & Svensson 2002; Stamps 2007; Wolf et al. 2007), and more work is clearly required to elucidate why particular combinations of behavioural types coexist and certain others do not. Furthermore, future work must address why particular types of explorative behaviour are structured into syndromes whereas others, in our case exploration of novel foods (Fig. 2), are not.

Conclusions

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Conclusions
  8. Acknowledgements
  9. References

In agreement with the predictions of the adaptive hypothesis for population variation in behavioural syndromes, we have shown that behavioural correlations covary consistently with aspects of the selective environment. These findings imply that behavioural syndromes are not fixed according to physiological and/or genetic constraints, and indicate that natural selection may have favoured the evolution of behavioural syndromes only in certain types of environment.

Acknowledgements

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Conclusions
  8. Acknowledgements
  9. References

Constructive criticisms on earlier versions of this work were made by Alison Bell, Felicity Huntingford, Kees van Oers, Anne Rutten and three anonymous referees. We thank Ashley Tweedale for capturing perch, the Countryside Council for Wales, Welsh Water, the Royal Society for the Protection of Birds, Jim Clark, Les Colley, Sally Ellis, Cliff Girling, Roger Goodier, Mrs Hadcock, Tristan Hatton, Mr Lucas, Ian Sims and Sir Richard Williams-Bulkeley for helpful advice during field work and permission to work on their properties, and Dick Visser for designing Figs 1 and 2. N. J. D. was supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO grants S 84–566 and 863·05·002).

References

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Conclusions
  8. Acknowledgements
  9. References
  • Armbruster, W.S. & Schwaegerle, K.E. (1996) Causes of covariation of phenotypic traits among populations. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 9, 261276.
  • Arnold, S.J. (1992) Constraints on phenotypic evolution. American Naturalist, 140, S85S107.
  • Bell, A.M. (2005) Behavioral differences between individuals and two populations of stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 18, 464473.
  • Bell, A.M. (2007) Future directions in behavioural syndromes research. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 274, 755761.
  • Bell, A.M. & Sih, A. (2007) Predation favours the evolution of personality in threespined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Ecology Letters in press.
  • Chandler, C.R. (1995) Practical considerations in the use of simultaneous inference for multiple tests. Animal Behaviour, 49, 524527.
  • Coleman, K. & Wilson, D.S. (1998) Shyness and boldness in pumpkinseed sunfish: individual differences are context specific. Animal Behaviour, 56, 927936.
  • Crawley, M.J. (1993) GLIM for Ecologists. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford.
  • Dall, S.R.X., Houston, A.I. & McNamara, J.M. (2004) The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective. Ecology Letters, 7, 734739.
  • Dingemanse, N.J., Both, C., Van Noordwijk, A.J., Rutten, A.L. & Drent, P.J. (2003) Natal dispersal and personalities in great tits (Parus major). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 270, 741747.
  • Dingemanse, N.J. & Réale, D. (2005) Natural selection and animal personality. Behaviour, 142, 11651190.
  • Gosling, S.D. (2001) From mice to men: what can we learn about personality from animal research? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 4586.
  • Hallgrímsson, B. & Hall, B.K. (2005) Variation. Elsevier Academic Press, Burlington.
  • Henderson, C.R. (1975) Best linear unbiased estimation and prediction under a selection model. Biometrics, 31, 423447.
  • Huntingford, F.A. (1976) The relationship between anti-predator behaviour and aggression among conspecifics in the three-spined stickleback, Gasterosteus aculeatus. Animal Behaviour, 24, 245260.
  • Hurlbert, S.H. (1984) Pseudoreplication and the design of ecological field experiments. Ecological Monographs, 54, 187211.
  • Krause, J. & Ruxton, G.D. (2002) Living in Groups. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
  • Lande, R. (1979) Quantitative genetics analysis of multivariate evolution, applied to brain: body size allometry. Evolution, 33, 402416.
  • Lande, R. (1986) The dynamics of peak shifts and the patterns of morphological evolution. Paleobiology, 12, 343354.
  • Lande, R. (1992) Neutral theory of quantitative genetic variance in an island model with local extinction and colonization. Evolution, 46, 381389.
  • Loeschke, V. (1987) Genetic Constraints on Adaptive Evolution. Springer Verlag, Berlin.
  • McElreath, R. & Strimling, P. (2006) How noisy information and individual asymmetries can make ‘personality’ an adaptation: a simple model. Animal Behaviour, 72, 11351139.
  • Moran, M.D. (2003) Arguments for rejecting the sequential Bonferroni in ecological studies. Oikos, 100, 403405.
  • Moretz, J.A., Martins, E.P. & Robison, D. (2007) Behavioral syndromes and the evolution of correlated behavior in zebrafish. Behavioral Ecology, 18, 556562.
  • Price, T. & Langen, T. (1992) Evolution of correlated characters. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 7, 307310.
  • Rasbash, J., Steele, F., Browne, W. & Prosser, B. (2004) A User's Guide to Mlwinn, version 2·0. Institute of Education, London.
  • Réale, D., Reader, S.M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. & Dingemanse, N.J. (2007) Integrating temperament in ecology and evolutionary biology. Biological Reviews, 82, 128.
  • Riechert, S.E. & Hedrick, A.V. (1993) A test of correlations among fitness-related behavioural traits in the spider, Agelenopsis aperta (Aranea, Agelinadae). Animal Behaviour, 46, 669675.
  • Sih, A. (1997) To hide or not to hide? Refuge use in a fluctuating environment. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 12, 375376.
  • Sih, A., Bell, A. & Johnson, J.C. (2004a) Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 19, 372378.
  • Sih, A., Bell, A.M., Johnson, J.C. & Ziemba, R.E. (2004b) Behavioural syndromes: an integrative overview. Quarterly Review of Biology, 79, 241277.
  • Sinervo, B. & Svensson, E. (2002) Correlational selection and the evolution of genomic architecture. Heredity, 89, 329338.
  • Sokal, R.R. & Rohlf, F.J. (1995) Biometry. W.H. Freeman, New York.
  • SPSS Inc. (1988) spssx User's Guide. SPSS Inc., Chicago.
  • Stamps, J.A. (2007) Growth-mortality tradeoffs and ‘personality traits’ in animals. Ecology Letters, 10, 355363.
  • Stirling, D.G., Réale, D. & Roff, D.A. (2002) Selection, structure and the heritability of behaviour. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 15, 277289.
  • Tabachnick, B.G. & Fidell, L.S. (2001) Using Multivariate Statistics. Allyn & Bacon, Boston, MA.
  • Walling, C.A., Dawnay, N., Kazem, A.J.N. & Wright, J. (2004) Predator inspection behaviour in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus): body size, local predation pressure and cooperation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 56, 164170.
  • Ward, A.J.W., Thomas, P., Hart, P.J.B. & Krause, J. (2004) Correlates of boldness in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 55, 561568.
  • Whitlock, M.C., Phillips, P.C. & Fowler, K. (2002) Persistence of changes in the genetic covariance matrix after a bottleneck. Evolution, 56, 19681975.
  • Wilson, D.S., Clark, A.B., Coleman, K. & Dearstyne, T. (1994) Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 9, 442446.
  • Wolf, M., Van Doorn, G.S., Leimar, O. & Weissing, F.J. (2007) Life-history trade-offs favour the evolution of animal personalities. Nature, 447, 581585.
  • Zar, J.H. (1999) Biostatistical Analyses. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ.