SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

Keywords:

  • Alces alces;
  • conception date;
  • litter size;
  • sex ratio

Summary

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials and methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References

1. The Trivers–Willard model of optimal sex ratios predicts that in polygynous species mothers in better condition should produce more male than female offspring. However, empirical support for this hypothesis in mammals and especially ungulates has been equivocal. This may be because the fitness of mothers has been defined in different ways, reflecting morphological, physiological or behavioural measures of condition. In addition, factors other than maternal condition can influence a mother’s fitness. Given that recent studies of wild ungulates have demonstrated the importance of the timing of conception and birth on offspring fitness, litters conceived at different stages of the rut might be expected to exhibit differences in types and embryonic sex ratio.

2. Based on a 6-year survey of the reproductive tracts of female moose harvested in Estonia, we investigated the effect of conception date on the types of litters produced and on the foetal sex ratio.

3. There was a clear relationship between conception date and litter characteristics. Overall, earlier conceived litters were more likely than those conceived late to contain multiple embryos and a high proportion of males. However, while foetal sex ratio varied nonlinearly with conception date in yearlings and subadults, no relationship was found in adults.

4. We conclude that female moose adjust foetal sex ratio and litter type/size depending on their age and the date of conception, and that these adjustments are in accordance with the Trivers–Willard hypothesis if females that conceive earlier are in better condition.


Introduction

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials and methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References

Trivers & Willard (1973) suggested that in polygynous mammals, where the fighting ability of males determines their access to females, the condition and hence reproductive success of males reflects the ability of their mothers to invest in them during the pre- and postnatal period. Consequently, mothers in good condition should produce an offspring sex ratio biased towards males (Trivers & Willard 1973). However, the degree to which this theoretical model is supported by empirical data from various taxa, including mammals, has been much debated (see references in Sheldon & West 2004).

First, a difficulty in testing the Trivers–Willard hypothesis in mammals is that theoretical work has indicated that the relationship between maternal condition and offspring sex ratio may be influenced by the characteristics of both male and female offspring (Leimar 1996). Specifically, if mothers can effectively transmit their condition to daughters – for instance, through inheritance of territories or social rank – this may outweigh the benefit of producing well-conditioned sons.

Second, the relationship between sex ratio and maternal condition may depend upon the chosen measure of condition. For example, a recent meta-analysis suggested that morphological and physiological measures collected post-conception give a poorer index of a mother’s ability to invest in offspring than evidence of behavioural dominance (Sheldon & West 2004). Hence, there is a need to identify appropriate measures of maternal fitness from among those traits that can be easily obtained from a large sample of animals in a range of environments.

We propose that the time of conception can be reflective of biased sex allocation at birth. A number of studies considering different ungulate species have shown that early conception and birth may be associated with higher fitness returns for mothers, owing to improved condition (Festa-Bianchet 1988; Holand et al. 2006) and reproductive performance in offspring (Albon, Clutton-Brock & Guinness 1987; Kruuk et al. 1999; Lindström 1999; Langvatn et al. 2004). Moreover, the positive effect of birth size on future survival (Loison, Langvatn & Solberg 1999) and reproductive success (Clutton-Brock, Albon & Guinness 1988; Kruuk et al. 1999) is usually greater in males than in females. Hence, it might be predicted that in highly dimorphic and polygynous ungulates, females that conceive earlier during the rut should produce a higher proportion of male offspring than those that conceive later (Kruuk et al. 1999). This hypothesis has gained support from an experimental study with female reindeer (Rangifer taranus L.) in Kaamanen Experimental Reindeer Station in Finland (Holand et al. 2006).

In contrast to reindeer and reed deer (Cervus elaphus L.), the litter size of moose (Alces alces L.) is not fixed: females usually produce one or two offspring (but occasionally more) during a single gestation period. In moose, the twinning rate (the average frequency of twins per female) is considered to be one of the most reliable indicators of reproductive ability (Solberg et al. 1999), which in turn has been found to be closely related to the age (Solberg et al. 1999) and body condition of females (Sæther & Haagenrud 1985; Sand 1996). To apply the Trivers–Willard hypothesis to species with variable litter sizes and modest cost differences between the production of male and female offspring, Williams (1979) proposed a model whereby females should change their litter size and sex ratio in relation to their own condition in the following sequence: ♀ < ♂ < ♀♀ < ♀♂ < ♂♂ < ♀♀♀ < ♀♀♂ and so on as their condition improves. However, this sequence of litter types can be different, when the cost difference between the embryonic sexes is large (Cassinello & Gomendio 1996) or if there is strong maternal transmission of condition to daughters (Leimar 1996).

In this study, we test the effect of conception date on the type of litters produced by female moose in Estonia. As an earlier conception date can be beneficial in terms of offspring fitness, we hypothesize that females that conceive earlier should produce a relatively higher proportion of expensive litters (male-biased litters with a higher twinning rate), compared with females that conceive later. In addition to Williams’s model (see above), we test other possible orderings of litter combinations, assuming that the relative costs of males and females stay the same in all litter types (see Materials and methods). Further, we look for overall differences in foetal sex ratios during different stages of the conception period. We also examine the effects of maternal age, body size, regional differences and population density on these parameters.

Materials and methods

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials and methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References

Study area

This study is based on data collected from female moose harvested throughout Estonia during the regular hunting season (1 October–30 November). Estonia experiences a climate that is midway between maritime and continental, with warm summers and fairly mild winters. The overall average annual air-temperature is 4·7 °C. The summer air-temperature is below the average for regions of Estonia’s latitude, whereas the winter air-temperature is considerably higher than average. Average annual precipitation in Estonia is 500–700 mm. Snow cover generally forms in mid-December and lasts until April (usually thickest at the end of February, 30–40 cm); however, in some mild winters, there is no permanent snow cover. The vegetation period (air-temperature > 5 °C) lasts 165–185 days, and the period of active vegetation (>10 °C) 110–135 days. Approximately 50% of Estonia is covered with coniferous forest (c. 26 000 km2).

To reflect the slight climatic, floristic and topographic differences between the different geographical regions of Estonia, the moose data gathered by wildlife boards were categorized as originating from the northern, western or south-eastern area, primarily based on the borders of administrative units (Fig. 1).

image

Figure 1.  The location of Estonia in north-east Europe and the regions of Estonia used in analyses. N, northern region; W, western region (including the three largest islands); SE, south-eastern region.

Download figure to PowerPoint

Data collection and measurements

The study was carried out in six successive years between 1999 and 2004. During this period, hunters collected the ovaries, uteri (with embryos if present) and lower jawbones of female moose. According to Estonian hunting traditions and legislation, hunted females are likely to represent a relatively random sample of the moose population. Hunters are instructed to collect reproductive tracts randomly from 20% to 30% of all hunted females per year. As the same rules of sampling were used by hunters in all three regions throughout the study period, we consider our sampling to be relatively random. As developing embryos become visible at the age of 3 weeks, and sexing is possible after the fourth week of development, we could include in our analyses a total of 289 samples with developing embryos (at least 4 weeks old) and a known date of harvest. However, in most analyses, we could use only data from 248 females, because in 41 cases the age of the female was unknown.

The age of females was determined as a function of tooth development and the degree of wear of molars and premolars (Knorre & Shubin 1959). Age was determined throughout the study period by the same person. Females were divided into five age groups: yearlings (1·5 years old), subadults (2·5 years old), subadults (3·5 years old), prime-aged (4·5–7·5 years old) and older (≥8·5 years old) individuals. In some analyses, we present separate models for younger (1·5–3·5 years old) and older (4·5 years old and older) individuals. It was expected that the cost of reproduction is likely to be the highest among younger individuals (yearlings and subadults –Clutton-Brock 1991) because they are more constrained by trade-offs between reproduction and future body growth (Sand 1996; Landete-Castillejos et al. 2004) in comparison with fully grown older individuals.

The number and age of embryos (with an accuracy of 1 week) were determined using methods described by Markgren (1969). Using the age of embryos and the culling date, a conception date was calculated with an accuracy of 1 week (expressed as the number of weeks since the beginning of August; first week, 1–7 August).

The sexing of embryos older than 6 weeks was based on visual inspection of secondary sexual characteristics (Markgren 1969). In the case of younger embryos (4–5 weeks old), an alternative method was employed. An embryonic tissue sample was taken and cells were fixed and stained on a slide with 0·005% methyl-blue. The samples were then examined within a few minutes on a microscope and scored for the presence or absence of a Barr body. A Barr body (also called sex chromatin) is the inactive X-chromosome in female somatic cells and can be detected as a densely staining body within the nucleus. A similar method was employed by King (1984). To determine the level of agreement between the two sexing methods, both were used to determine the sex of 15 older embryos and were found to give identical results.

Moose density

Moose abundance and hunting rates were reported by wildlife boards throughout the species range in Estonia. Density estimates were based on counts of tracks carried out at the end of winter before parturition (official counts). Density was calculated as the number of individuals per 1000 ha of suitable moose habitat (c. 24 000 km2 in Estonia). Estimates of moose density are comparable between years because census data were gathered throughout the study period using identical methods. The density of the wintering moose population in Estonia varied from 3·2 to 5·4 individuals per 1000 ha during the study period.

Conception period

In all Holarctic moose populations, the main mating period is believed to last 20–22 days and fall within the two last weeks of September and the first week of October (Knorre 1959; Lent 1974; Filonov 1983). However, the heating of cows may last up to 12 weeks, from mid-August to the first week of November (Lent 1974; Sigouin, Ouellet & Courtois 1995; Bubenik 1998). The length of this period is related to the recurrent oestrus cycles of females, i.e. non-fertilization is followed by the next oestrus cycle in 3–4 weeks (Kozlo 1983; Schwartz 1998).

During our 6-year study, the majority (83%) of studied moose cows were conceived within a period of 4 weeks: between the sixth and ninth week (5 September–2 October), where week 1 is the first week of August (Fig. 2). The peak (mode and also median) of conception occurred during the eighth week, when one-third of all conceptions took place. The mean conception date was 7·6 weeks (SE = 0·10), whereas the most extreme dates were the 2nd (8–14 August) and 12th weeks (17–23 October, Fig. 2).

image

Figure 2.  Conception dates (in weeks) of studied females.

Download figure to PowerPoint

Data analysis

All data analysis was performed using sas 9 (glmm; SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC, USA) and statistica 8 (Statsoft Inc., Tulsa, OK, USA). The effect of conception date on the type of litter produced was studied using ordinal multinomial logistic regression. Five litter types were identified based on Williams’s (1979) model (Model 1), as described in the Introduction (1 – the cheapest litter with one female embryo and 5 – the most expensive with two male embryos). As there were only three occasions during our study when females produced more than two embryos, we excluded these data from our analyses. We also tested for alternative orderings of litter combinations, assuming that the relative costs of males and females stay the same in all litter types. The respective changes in litter size and sex ratio in relation to the improvement of female condition are as follows: Model 2: ♀ < ♀♀ < ♂ < ♂♀ < ♂♂; Model 3: ♂ < ♀ < ♂♂ < ♂♀ < ♀♀; Model 4: ♂ < ♂♂ < ♀ < ♂♀ < ♀♀. All initial models included the age of the mother, the year and the region as categorical factors, and conception date, population density and lower jawbone length as continuous predictors (note that population density and jawbone length were omitted from all final models due to their insignificance). We also tested for possible interactions between mother age and other factors in the model. The effect of conception date on the size of litters was studied using binomial logistic regression (0 – single, 1 – twins).

To investigate the differences in embryonic sex ratios, two approaches were used. First, we applied ordinal multinomial logistic regression (1 – one or two female embryos, 2 – one male and one female embryo, 3 – one or two male embryos). In this case, conception date was used as a continuous predictor in the model. We did not examine sex ratios using binomial logistic regression (0 – female embryo, 1– male embryo) with female as a random factor because nearly 50% of females had only one embryo (i.e. no within-group replication for these females). The goodness-of-fit of logistic regression models with categorical response variables was tested using the Pearson chi2 or Deviance statistic. The ratios of these statistics to the respective degrees of freedom were close to 1 in all cases. Thus, there was no evidence of overdispersion, suggesting that the values of the parameter estimates were appropriately scaled. Due to the nonlinear relationship between sex ratio and conception date, we also used another approach. We divided conception period into three stages based on the peaks (median) in conception rate for each year separately. Thereafter, chi-squared tests were applied separately within each stage. The stages were classified as follows: first (pre-peak) stage – the period prior to the week of peak conception; second (peak) stage – the week of peak conception, when nearly one-third of all females were fertilized (eighth week in four and seventh week in two study years); and third (post-peak) stage – the period after the week of peak conception. We also investigated deviations from a 1 : 1 sex ratio in different age groups within each conception stage.

Results

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials and methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References

Conception date significantly affected the type of litter conceived (Table 1). In agreement with Williams’s (1979) model (the cheapest litter with one female embryo and the most expensive with two male embryos), females that conceived earlier had a higher probability of producing more expensive litters than those conceived later in the season (Table 1). Other possible orderings of litter types were not significantly related to the conception date (Table 1). Female age and region were also significant predictors of litter type (Table 1). Among younger age groups (1·5- to 3·5-year-old individuals), the ‘cheapest’ litter with one female embryo was the most frequent litter type (36–51%), whereas in prime-aged females (4·5–7·5 years old) male–female twins were most frequent (39%), and in older females (≥8·5 years old) female–female twins were most frequent (28%, Table 2). Among regions, the mean litter value increased from the western to the south-eastern and northern areas (mean regional values 2·30, 2·60 and 2·98 respectively).

Table 1.   The influence of conception date on the type of litter conceived by female moose in Estonia during 1999–2004 (N = 248; ordinal logistic regression, F – female, M – male; four possible models are presented)
FactorD.f.Wald StatP-valueParameter estimateSE of estimate
  1. Model 1 represents Williams’s (1979) model with litter types F, M, FF, MF, MM; the cheapest litter including one female embryo and the most expensive including two male embryos. Other possible orderings based on the relative cost of males and females are as follows: Model 2 – F, FF, M, MF, MM; Model 3 – M, F, MM, MF, FF; Model 4 – M, MM, F, MF and FF.

Model 1
 Conception date15·970·015−0·210·09
 Age429·57<0·001  
 Region211·520·003  
 Year52·210·8  
Model 2
 Conception date12·370·12−0·120·08
 Age419·64<0·001  
 Region27·880·019  
 Year53·120·68  
Model 3
 Conception date12·620·10−0·130·08
 Age420·93<0·001  
 Region25·330·07  
 Year53·250·66  
Model 4
 Conception date11·320·25−0·090·08
 Age410·850·028  
 Region24·020·13  
 Year53·870·57  
Table 2.   Overview of twinning rates, the proportion of male embryos and the proportional distribution of different types of litter categories among different age groups in our study
Age group (years)N% of twins% of male embryosLitter categories
♀♀♀♂♂♂
  1. Values in parentheses are percentages.

1·54115·633·321 (51)13 (32)4 (10)3 (7)0 (0)
2·56439·139·324 (37)15 (23)10 (16)10 (16)5 (8)
3·55046·038·418 (36)9 (18)10 (20)7 (14)6 (12)
4·5–7·55766·738·912 (21)7 (12)12 (21)22 (39)4 (7)
≥8·53675·050·82 (6)7 (19)10 (28)9 (25)8 (22)
Unknown4141·539·713 (32)11 (27)8 (19)6 (15)3 (7)
Total28947·440·190 (31)62 (21)54 (19)57 (20)26 (9)

In agreement with Williams’s model, conception date also predicted the twinning rate. The results of the binomial logistic regression indicated that females that conceived earlier had more twins (conception date: Wald Stat = 5·28, d.f. = 1, P = 0·021, Estimate ± SE: −0·28 ± 0·12), as did older females (age: Wald Stat = 18·93, d.f. = 4, P < 0·001). There were also regional (mean number of embryos per female: 1·60, 1·57 and 1·36, respectively, in northern, south-eastern and western regions), but not inter-annual differences in the twinning rate (region: Wald Stat = 9·80, d.f. = 2, P = 0·007; year: Wald Stat = 0·35, d.f. = 5, P = 0·6).

The overall foetal sex ratio (pooled data of all studied females) differed significantly from 1 : 1 ratio (1·5 female per one male, N = 437; χ2 = 8·74; P = 0·0031), only 40·1% of all detected embryos were males (Table 2).

Foetal sex ratio varied nonlinearly with the progress of the conception period, with the proportion of male embryos highest in the first stage (Table 3). Moreover, a nearly significant age by conception date interaction emerged (Table 3). Further analysis revealed that in the case of yearlings and subadults, the best-fit model included the third degree polynomial relationship between the sex ratio and conception date (Fig. 3, Table 3), whereas no linear or nonlinear relationship was found in adults (Table 3). The body size index was unrelated to litter type and foetal sex ratio and was omitted from all final models.

Table 3.   The influence of conception date on the foetal sex ratio in female moose in Estonia during 1999–2004 (ordinal logistic regression, categories of foetal sex ratio per female: 1 – one or two female embryos, 2 – one male and one female embryo, 3 – one or two male embryos)
FactorD.f.Wald Stat P-valueParameter estimateSE of estimate
  1. In addition to the full model, separate models are presented for younger (1·5–3·5 years old) and older (4·5 years old and older) individuals. Conception daten indicates an nth degree polynomial.

Conception date14·230·0393·471·69
Conception date215·400·020−0·560·24
Conception date316·250·0120·0280·011
Age46·060·19  
Year51·600·9  
Age × conception date48·860·066  
1·5–3·5 Years old
 Conception date14·630·0314·021·87
 Conception date216·230·012−0·660·27
 Conception date317·200·0070·0320·012
 Year52·110·83  
>3·5 Years old
 Conception date12·030·150·220·12
 Year51·810·87  
image

Figure 3.  Temporal changes in the proportion of male embryos produced by yearlings and subadults (1·5–3·5 years old). Least-square relationship is given (without accounting for year).

Download figure to PowerPoint

Owing to the nonlinear relationship between sex ratio and conception date, we also used the alternative approach of dividing the conception period into three consecutive stages and examining sex ratio separately within each (see Materials and methods). The overall foetal sex ratio (all females included) appeared to differ significantly between the different stages. In the first stage, the number of female and male embryos was almost equal (0·9 females per male, respectively, N = 159; χ2 = 0·38; P = 0·54). During the subsequent stages, the foetal sex ratio was strongly skewed towards females: 2·5 : 1 in the second stage (N = 145; χ2 = 13·47; P = 0·0002); and 1·8 : 1 in the third stage (N = 133; χ2 = 5·23; P = 0·022).

The foetal sex ratio of yearlings and subadults differed considerably between the different conception stages (Fig. 3). During the first stage, the embryonic sex ratio (0·7 females per one male) did not differ significantly from the balanced (1 : 1) sex ratio (N = 78; χ2 = 1·27; P = 0·26). The embryonic sex ratio was most strongly female biased (3·2 : 1) in the second stage of conception (N = 71; χ2 = 10·40; P = 0·0013), and still moderately female biased (2·5 : 1) in the third stage (N = 69; χ2 = 6·42; P = 0·011).

In adults (prime-aged and older females), no significant differences from the balanced ratio were found in foetal sex ratios during any of the conception stages: 1·6 : 1 in the first stage (N = 52; χ2 = 1·4; P = 0·24); 1·4 : 1 in the second stage (N = 52; χ2 = 0·62; P = 0·43); and 1·1 : 1 in the third stage (N = 57; χ2 = 0·08; P = 0·78).

Discussion

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials and methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References

According to Williams’s (1979) model, the females in best condition should produce two sons; those in slightly poorer condition should produce one son and one daughter, then two daughters, and then a son, whereas females in the poorest condition should produce a daughter. We demonstrate that other female characteristics, such as the date of conception, are also good predictors of the ordering of litter types: earlier conceived females more frequently produced expensive litters (male-biased litters with a high twinning rate) in comparison with later conceived females. However, it remains to be established whether the mechanism of sex allocation is directly related to conception date per se, or whether the effect of conception date is mediated by maternal condition.

Why should females that conceive early bias their foetal sex ratio in favour of males? It has been shown that an early start in life can be beneficial in terms of offspring fitness – higher body mass at birth and in the first autumn (Holand et al. 2006), a higher probability of survival (Festa-Bianchet 1988; Langvatn et al. 2004) and a higher lifetime reproductive success (Kruuk et al. 1999). Given that the effect of birth size on future survival (Loison et al. 1999) and reproductive success (Clutton-Brock et al. 1988; Kruuk et al. 1999) is more pronounced in males (due to sexual selection) than in females, sons that are born earlier are relatively more successful than earlier-born daughters.

The negative effects of delayed calving might be reduced to some extent if extra resources could be allocated to offspring during the lactation period or if the period of maternal care could be prolonged (review in Gaillard et al. 2000). However, an increase in investment during lactation, which is considered to be the most energy demanding period in the reproductive cycle (Clutton-Brock 1991), would probably further reduce the fertility and probability of survival of the mother (review in Mysterud, Coulson & Stenseth 2002). Hence, fitness returns to mothers can be increased not only by adjusting the foetal sex ratio, but also by advancing conception as a strategy to minimize the postpartum costs of reproduction.

It is noteworthy that the effect of conception date on sex ratio (more male embryos in the first stage of conception) was significant only among yearlings (1·5 years old) and subadults (2·5 and 3·5 years old), but not among prime-aged and older adults. This finding is somewhat contrary to our initial predictions. However, the costs of reproduction are indeed considered to be the highest among younger individuals (Clutton-Brock 1991) because young animals are more constrained by a trade-off between reproduction and future body growth (Sand 1996; Landete-Castillejos et al. 2004) in comparison with fully grown individuals. Accordingly, pre-weaning maternal care has been found to increase as the age of the mother increases (Weladji et al. 2003). This suggests that prime-aged and older females may be able to compensate for giving birth to their offspring late in the season by increasing their maternal care with relatively little cost, compared with younger individuals. Hence, it is likely that the effect of advancement of conception (and hence birth) is more important among younger age groups. Second, we cannot exclude the possibility that foetal sex ratio and the type of litter produced by prime-aged and older females remained relatively constant throughout the conception period because such females mated with higher-than-average quality males.

Besides conception date, female age was also linked to the type of litter conceived. This finding concurs with the many previous studies that have shown a strong effect of female age on reproductive effort, with prime-aged and older females producing the highest number of offspring (Sand 1996; Solberg et al. 1999) and the highest rate of males calves (Côté & Festa-Bianchet 2001,Landete-Castillejos et al. 2004; Säde 2004). Hence, if age is indicative of condition, then these observations are in agreement with the Trivers–Willard theory. However, in our study, female age itself was not a significant predictor of foetal sex ratio, suggesting that a complex relationship exists between litter type and sex ratio in species that produce a variable litter size.

Although the majority of the moose cows (83%) in our study were conceived during a period of 4 weeks, from mid-September up to mid-October, the whole conception period was extended over a period of 12 weeks (taking all years together). It has been shown for moose that a long conception period may reflect a predominance of cows in the population (Lent 1974; Sigouin et al. 1995; Bubenik 1998). Indeed, the sex ratio in the Estonian moose population has been female biased during the last 20 years, varying from 1·3 to 1·6 females per male (Tõnisson 2005). During the last decade, the mean age of males and females has declined sharply in the Estonian moose population due to the high reproductive rates that have accompanied the increasing population size, and due to human exploitation. We propose that the skew in the overall embryonic sex ratio and the long duration of the conception period in Estonian moose population are caused by a deficit of prime-aged males. In this respect, it has been shown that young males produce less male offspring (Sæther et al. 2004), they enter the rut later compared with older males (Clutton-Brock, Rose & Guiness 1997; Yoccoz et al. 2002), and females mate with them only when the prime-aged dominant males are not present (Komers, Birgersson & Ekvall 1999). The deficit of high-quality males is probably most obvious during the peak period of the conception period when the number of females in heat is highest. This may explain the extremely biased foetal sex ratio among younger females (three females per male) that conceived during the peak conception phase (median week) in our study.

We also found significant regional effects on the type and size of litters produced. These differences can most likely be explained by local variations in population density. While litter sizes increased from western towards south-eastern and northern areas, regional densities showed the opposite trend: 0·45 ind. km−2 in western, 0·39 ind. km−2 in south-eastern and 0·37 ind. km−2 in northern areas (Veeroja et al. 2008). Hence, the negative effects of increased density may be explained by increased competition for food, as has previously been reported from several ungulates in different geographical regions (Weladji & Holand 2003). It is also possible that slight regional differences in climatic conditions (e.g. the more maritime climate in western areas) may have had some influence.

In conclusion, we established that foetal sex ratio and litter investment vary in relation to conception date and the age of the mother. However, as the positive effect of advanced conception on foetal sex ratio was evident only in yearlings (1·5 years old) and subadults (2·5 and 3·5 years old), it is likely that prime-aged and older females adopt more flexible reproductive strategies by being more selective in their choice of mates and by investing more into postnatal maternal care.

Acknowledgements

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials and methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References

We thank John Davison, Margo Tannik, Tanel Türna, Leeni Seppel, Raivo Mänd, Tiit Randveer and Peep Männil for their help. Financial support for this study was provided by the Estonian Environmental Investment Centre, the Estonian Science Foundation (grant number 8376 to V.T.), the Estonian Ministry of Education and Science (target-financing project number 0180004s09) and the European Union through the European Regional Development Fund (Center of Excellence FIBIR).

References

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials and methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References
  • Albon, S.D., Clutton-Brock, T.H. & Guinness, F.E. (1987) Early development and population dynamics in red deer. II. Density-independent effects and cohort variation. Journal of Animal Ecology, 56, 6981.
  • Bubenik, A.B. (1998) Behavior. Ecology and Management of the North American Moose (eds A.W.Franzmann & C.C.Schwartz), pp. 141171. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington/London.
  • Cassinello, J. & Gomendio, M. (1996) Adaptive variation in litter size and sex ratio at birth in a sexually dimorphic ungulate. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 263, 14611466.
  • Clutton-Brock, T.H. (1991) The evolution of parental care. Monographs in Behavior and Ecology. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
  • Clutton-Brock, T.H., Albon, S.D. & Guinness, F.E. (1988) Reproductive success in male and female red deer. Reproductive Success (ed. T.H.Clutton-Brock), pp. 325343. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
  • Clutton-Brock, T.H., Rose, K.E. & Guiness, F.E. (1997) Density-related changes in sexual selection in red deer. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 264, 15091516.
  • Côté, S.D. & Festa-Bianchet, M. (2001) Offspring sex ratio in relation to maternal age and social rank in mountain goats (Oreamnos americanus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 49, 260265.
  • Festa-Bianchet, M. (1988) Birthdate and lamb survival in bighorn lambs (Ovis Canadensis). Journal of Zoology, 214, 653661.
  • Filonov, K.P. (1983) Losj (in Russian). Lesnaja Promyshlennost. Moskva.
  • Gaillard, J.M., Festa-Bianchet, M., Yoccoz, N.G., Loison, A. & Toigo, C. (2000) Temporal variation in fitness components and population dynamics of large herbivores. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 31, 367393.
  • Holand, Ø., Mysterud, A., Røed, K.H., Coulson, T., Gjøstein, H., Weladji, R.B. & Nieminen, M. (2006) Adaptive adjustment of offspring sex ratio and maternal reproductive effort in an iteroparous mammal. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 273, 293299.
  • King, W.A. (1984) Sexing embryos by cytological methods. Theriogenology, 21, 717.
  • Knorre, E.P. (1959) Ekologia losja (in Russian). Trudy Pećhoro-Ilyćskogo Gosudarstvennogo Zapovednika. Syktyvkar, Vol. 7, pp. 5122.
  • Knorre, E.P. & Shubin, G.G. (1959) Opredelenie vozrasta losja (in Russian). Trudy Pećhoro-Ilyćskogo Gosudarstvennogo Zapovednika. Syktyvkar, Vol. 7, pp., 123132.
  • Komers, P.E., Birgersson, B. & Ekvall, K. (1999) Timing of estrus in fallow deer is adjusted to the age of available mates. The American Naturalist, 153, 431436.
  • Kozlo, P.G. (1983) The Ecologo-morphological Analysis of Moose Populations. Nauka I Technika Press, Minsk (in Russian).
  • Kruuk, L.E.B., Clutton-Brock, T.H., Rose, K.E. & Guinness, F.E. (1999) Early determinants of lifetime reproductive success differ between the sexes in red deer. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 266, 16551661.
  • Landete-Castillejos, T., Gortázar, C., Vincente, J., Fierro, Y., Garcia, A. & Gallego, L. (2004) Age-related foetal sex ratio bias in Iberian red deer (Cervus elaphus hispanicus): are male calves too expensive for growing mothers? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 56, 18.
  • Langvatn, R., Mysterud, A., Stenseth, N.C. & Yoccoz, N.G. (2004) Timing and synchrony of ovulation in red deer constrained by short northern summers. The American Naturalist, 163, 763772.
  • Leimar, O. (1996) Life-history analysis of the Trivers and Willard problem. Behavioral Ecology, 7, 316325.
  • Lent, P.C. (1974) A review of rutting behavior in moose. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Moose Ecology Held in Quebec City, Canada, 27–28 Mars 1973 (ed. J.Bedard). Le Naturaliste Canadien, 101, 307323.
  • Lindström, J. (1999) Early development and fitness in birds and mammals. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 14, 343348.
  • Loison, A., Langvatn, R. & Solberg, E.J. (1999) Body mass and winter mortality in red deer calves: disentangling sex and climatic effects. Ecography, 22, 2030.
  • Markgren, G. (1969) Reproduction of moose in Sweden. Viltrevy, 6, 127299.
  • Mysterud, A., Coulson, T. & Stenseth, N.C. (2002) The role of males in the dynamics of ungulate population (Essay rewiew). Journal of Animal Ecology, 71, 907915.
  • Säde, S. (2004) Twinning rate and foetal sex ratio of moose Alces alces in Estonia during low-density and increasing population phases in relation to maternal age. Wildlife Biology, 10, 295300.
  • Sæther, B.-E. & Haagenrud, H. (1985) Life history of the moose (Alces alces): relationship between growth and reproduction. Holarctic Ecology, 8, 100106.
  • Sæther, B.-E., Solberg, E.J., Heim, M., Stacy, J.E., Jakobsen, K. & Olstad, R. (2004) Offspring sex ratio in moose Alces alces in relation to paternal age: an experiment. Wildlife Biology, 10, 5157.
  • Sand, H. (1996) Life history patterns in female moose (Alces alces): the relationship between age, body size, fecundity and environmental conditions. Oecologia, 106, 212220.
  • Schwartz, C.C. (1998) Reproduction, natality and growth. Ecology and Management of the North American Moose (eds A.W.Franzmann & C.C.Schwartz), pp. 141171. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington/London.
  • Sheldon, B.C. & West, S.A. (2004) Maternal dominance, maternal condition, and offspring sex ratio in ungulate mammals. The American Naturalist, 163, 4054.
  • Sigouin, D., Ouellet, J.P. & Courtois, R. (1995) Moose (Alces alces) rutting period variations. Alces, 31, 185197.
  • Solberg, E.J., Saether, B.-E., Strand, O. & Loison, A. (1999) Dynamics of a harvested moose population in a variable environment. Journal of Animal Ecology, 68, 186204.
  • Tõnisson, J. (2005) Management of Estonian moose population in 2004 (in Estonian). Project Report to Estonian Ministry of Environment. Centre of Forest Protection and Silviculture, Tartu.
  • Trivers, R.L. & Willard, D.E. (1973) Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science, 179, 9092.
  • Veeroja, R., Tilgar, V., Kirk, A. & Tõnisson, J. (2008) Climatic effects of life-history traits of moose in Estonia. Oecologia, 154, 703713.
  • Weladji, R.B. & Holand, Ø. (2003) Global climate change and reindeer: effects of winter weather on the autumn weight and growth of calves. Oecologia, 136, 317323.
  • Weladji, R.B., Holand, Ø., Steinheim, G. & Lenvik, D. (2003) Sex-specific preweaning maternal care in reindeer (Rangifer tarandus t.). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 53, 308314.
  • Williams, G.C. (1979) The question of adaptive variation in sex ratio in outcrossed vertebrates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 205, 567580.
  • Yoccoz, N.G., Mysterud, A., Langvatn, R. & Stenseth, N.C. (2002) Age- and density-dependent reproductive effort in male reed deer. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 269, 15231529.