SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

Keywords:

  • food web;
  • Fucus serratus;
  • meso-herbivores;
  • periwinkles;
  • phenotypic plasticity;
  • plant–herbivore interactions;
  • seaweed recruitment;
  • trait-mediated;
  • trophic interaction;
  • waterborne info-chemicals

Summary

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials & methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References

1. Non-consumptive predator effects are an important driver in predator–prey interactions. While indirect effects of predation risk shape interspecific species interactions, e.g. in fuelling trophic cascades, knowledge about non-consumptive predator effects on intraspecific interactions among intimidated prey is scarce.

2. In laboratory experiments, we tested whether predation by male and female green crabs (Carcinus maenas) is stronger on small than on larger specimens of the periwinkle Littorina littorea, an important herbivore species in intertidal habitats of the temperate zone. Moreover, we asked whether size-specific differences in predation-induced mortality reflect as a stronger effect of predation risk (caused by nearby consuming crabs) on the foraging behaviour of more threatened periwinkles and ultimately on the recruitment success of the brown seaweed Fucus serratus. Furthermore, predator activity and prey abundance was measured in the field and the efficacy of predation risk on the survival of seaweed recruits was assessed in three field experiments with two different set-ups (open plots and crab exclosures).

3. Male crabs consumed smaller periwinkles more often than larger conspecifics, while females refused periwinkles as food. In summer, 80–90% of periwinkles in the field attained a size at which crab predation was minimal in laboratory assays. The observed up to 15-fold decrease in the consumption of seaweed recruits by periwinkles in the presence of nearby consuming crabs in laboratory assays was, however, independent of the size of periwinkles. Predation risk effects from the laboratory were only confirmed in field experiments using crab exclosures but not with open plots, suggesting an artefact due to trespassing by wild crabs in the open plot set-up.

4.Synthesis. Predation risk may not change intraspecific competition for food in L. littorea. Yet, risk effects on foraging behaviour of periwinkles that experience a low threat of real predation indicate that non-consumptive predator effects may affect prey population dynamics more strongly than consumptive predator effects. Thus predation risk effects may be indirectly beneficial for the survival of basal species like perennial seaweeds, as experimental evidence from field and laboratory experiments indicates.


Introduction

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials & methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References

Predicting patterns of species composition at the community and ecosystem level is the prime goal of ecology. Therefore, knowledge about the mode of how species interact is a fundamental prerequisite. Past research often investigated pair-wise species interactions, revealing the function of direct effects like competitive exclusion among species (e.g. Fine & Loya 2003). However, under natural conditions biological interactions occur among multiple species, permitting indirect effects. The latter can strongly inflate ecosystem complexity as indirect effects may dampen or even reverse the direct effects in the interaction between two species, often with strong effects on basal species like plants and seaweeds (reviewed e.g. in Wootton 2002; Hay et al. 2004; Ohgushi 2005; Creel & Christianson 2008).

Studies on density-mediated indirect effects, which fuel, for instance, trophic cascades (Lubchenco 1978; Simenstad, Estes & Kenyon 1978), have profoundly advanced our understanding of the role that consumptive effects have on the structure of communities and ecosystems. In addition, non-consumptive predator effects may result from the presence of risk cues, which are known to affect the behaviour, morphology and life history of prey (e.g. Ferrari, Wisenden & Chivers 2010). Schmitz et al. (2008) proposed generally opposing indirect predator effects on ecosystem properties and functions between non-consumptive and consumptive predator effects. This suggests that consideration of prey responses to predation risk may inflate our ability to explain contingencies in and the modulation of trophic interactions better than by addressing predation induced changes in prey density alone. In fact, classic examples of consumptive indirect effects in species interactions seem to result at least partially from the impact of intimidation on prey (Peckarsky et al. 2008; but see Kauffman, Brodie & Jules 2010). Non-consumptive predator effects should be more effective than consumptive effects because the latter attenuate through food chains, while the former remain strong (Preisser, Bolnick & Benard 2005), function immediately, and do both for the entire period that a predator is present (Werner & Peacor 2003).

Non-consumptive predator effects may be of particular importance in structuring marine ecosystems as the effects of intimidation, rather than those of consumption, are stronger in aquatic than in terrestrial ecosystems (Preisser, Bolnick & Benard 2005). The available evidence from marine systems suggests that non-consumptive predator effects can (i) shape patterns in prey distribution (Rochette & Dill 2000), (ii) enhance prey survival (Flynn & Smee 2010) and (iii) alter prey population dynamics and energy flow by changing mating success in prey (Koch, Lynch & Rochette 2007), reducing prey susceptibility to consumers (Yun et al. 2007), and/or changing feeding behaviour of intermediate consumers (Trussell, Ewanchuk & Bertness 2002). Less is known, however, about the influence of individual properties like sex, age or size of both predators and prey on strength and temporal variation of predation risk. Intraspecific variation in the ability to release or in the susceptibility to risk cues may increase the variability in the outcome of species interactions. Experimental evidence about sources for intraspecific variation in risk strength is scarce. According to Spooner, Coleman & Attrill (2007), the gastropods Gibbula umbilicalis and Littorina littorea showed no differences in their responses to risk cues originating from males and females of the predatory crab Carcinus maenas. In contrast, the level of satiation in juvenile prey (Nucella lapillus) determined their response to C. maenas risk cues (Vadas, Burrows & Hughes 1994). Prey size may be another source for intraspecific variation in the magnitude of risk effects. Optimal foraging theory predicts prey-size-dependent consumption rates of predators. Experimental evidence comes, for instance, from benthic ecosystems where C. maenas prefers blue mussels (Mytilus edulis) of intermediate size several-fold over smaller and larger conspecifics (Enderlein et al. 2003). Hence, size-specificity in predation-induced mortality may result in intraspecific variation of risk effects in prey, like herbivores, and thus concomitant shifts in the competition among conspecific prey individuals for resources, like seaweeds and plants, seem possible. Experimental evidence that predator traits can indirectly alter intraspecific and interspecific competition in prey is, to our knowledge, only available for freshwater food webs. There, the presence of nearby predators reduced the activity level and thus growth rate of small, but not of large, larvae of conspecific and heterospecific frog species (Peacor & Werner 2000). Furthermore, information on the seasonal variation in non-consumptive predator effects is scarce (but see Jacobsen & Stabell 1999), yet required for predicting the temporal scales at which, for instance, trait-mediated indirect interactions will be effective in seasonally-driven ecosystems, like those in the temperate zone.

This study addressed the effects of predation risk on changes in prey feeding behaviour in a temperate rocky intertidal food chain, using the predatory green crab C. maenas (L.), the herbivorous periwinkle L. littorea (L.), and recruits of the brown seaweed Fucus serratus (L.). Trussell, Ewanchuk & Bertness (2002) demonstrated that nearby consuming C. maenas had a strong, positive, indirect effect on the survival of seaweed recruits on a NW Atlantic rocky intertidal shore by changing periwinkle foraging behaviour. Patterns in trait-mediated interactions may, however, vary considerately across the Atlantic, like it has been demonstrated for phenotypic plastic responses in seaweed–grazer interactions (Long & Trussell 2007). This simple food chain also exists on NE Atlantic shores, where L. littorea may reach densities of > 400 individuals m−2 (Eschweiler, Molis & Buschbaum 2009), a level at which intraspecific competition may occur in L. littorea (Petraitis 2002). Yet, knowledge on trait-mediated indirect interactions in this food chain is missing for the NE Atlantic and their potential in affecting intraspecific competition in periwinkles has not been studied altogether. In laboratory experiments, we tested with NE Atlantic organisms at two times (i) whether the consumption of L. littorea by C. maenas was size-dependent; (ii) whether L. littorea consumed less F. serratus recruits in the presence than in the absence of consuming C. maenas, and (iii) whether changes in L. littorea foraging behaviour to predation risk were dependent on the size of L. littorea. Furthermore, we assessed in manipulative experiments whether findings from laboratory assays hold under field conditions.

Materials & methods

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials & methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References

Study site

Field experiments were conducted and the organisms used in laboratory experiments were collected in the mid-intertidal at Nordwatt Helgoland, Germany (54°11′ N, 7°11′ E), NE Atlantic. Nordwatt is a moderately wave-exposed intertidal platform with semi-diurnal tides of an average range of 2.5 m. The emergent substratum consists of red sandstone, scarcely scattered flint boulders (max. Ø 25 cm) and small patches of shell. A previously reported mussel bed (Janke 1986) at the study site had disappeared. Presently, few mussels exist there, with specimens <40 mm hiding in crevices and those >40 mm attaching to rock surfaces (M. Molis, pers. obs.). The latter attain a size of reduced vulnerability to C. maenas predation (Enderlein et al. 2003). The mid-intertidal is characterized by two persistent stages of a Fucus-habitat (Bartsch & Tittley 2004). One area is dominated by a dense canopy of F. serratus, while the other area, where the field experiment was conducted, is 30% covered by F. serratus, F. vesiculosus and F. spiralis (hereafter Littorina Barren). Fucus-associated understorey algae and invertebrates exist in both areas with the periwinkle L. littorea as the dominant herbivore occurring year-round at the study site (see Bartsch & Tittley 2004; Reichert et al. 2008 for details on species composition). In contrast, C. maenas is absent from Nordwatt between late October and March (M. Molis, pers. obs.), when it is located in the relatively warmer subtidal habitats until water temperature reaches 6–8 °C again in spring (D. Klings, pers. comm.).

Experimental design and set-up

Activity of C. maenas and abundance of L. littorea

To characterize activity patterns of C. maenas and the population structure of L. littorea, the abundance of both was recorded at Littorina Barren. To estimate the activity of C. maenas, one non-baited trap was set up during low tide at 6 days between 13 June and 11 September 2008, and at 30 days between 10 June and 28 July 2009. After 24 h of deployment, number and sex of trapped C. maenas were recorded and their carapace width measured to the nearest 0.01 mm with digital callipers. The density of L. littorea was first estimated in September 2007 and hereafter about every 3 months until September 2008. At each sampling date, all L. littorea specimens were collected from 12 randomly chosen 1 m2 large non-permanent plots and transported to the laboratory of the Biologische Anstalt Helgoland. Here, the size of each specimen was measured with digital callipers to the nearest 0.01 mm as the maximum distance of the shell parallel to the plane of the aperture. The abundance of C. maenas of different sex was compared with a paired t-test from 36 daily measurements. Using a two-way repeated-measure anova, the density of L. littorea of different size classes encountered per square metre (within-subject factor, fixed, four levels) was compared among seasons (grouping factor, random, five levels).

Consumption rates of C. maenas

To assess whether C. maenas prefers L. littorea of a certain size and whether this preference was sex-dependent, one multi-choice feeding assay was conducted in the laboratory from 6 to 20 September 2007 (late summer) and two multi-choice assays between 18 June and 22 July 2008 (early summer). Two days before the start of the experiment in 2007, single male C. maenas (mean ± SD carapace width 68.13 ± 5.07 mm) were introduced to feeding arenas without food. As feeding arenas we used transparent plastic aquaria (19 × 17.5 × 32.5 cm), filled, on average, with 8.1 L of seawater. Each aquarium ( = experimental unit, EU) was individually supplied with a uni-directional flow of ambient filtered (sand and cotton) and aerated seawater (16.5 ° and 18 °C in early and late summer, respectively) at a rate of c. 4 L h−1. Seawater was directly pumped from the nearby (< 50 m) North Sea into the laboratory at the Biologische Anstalt Helgoland. At the beginning of the 2007 experiment, 20 feeding arenas were populated with four L. littorea from each of the four size classes (i) <18 mm, (ii) 18 to <20 mm, (iii) 20 to <23 mm, and (iv) ≥23 mm (total of 16 periwinkles per EU). In 2008, the experiment was repeated with male C. maenas (68.67 ± 3.88 mm) and extended by 20 additional feeding arenas, to which L. littorea were added as in the 2007 experiment, but which contained one female C. maenas (55.89 ± 8.27 mm) each. The number of consumed L. littorea per size category was recorded daily during the 14-day-long feeding assays. From each EU where C. maenas was feeding, the total number of L. littorea of different size classes that where consumed during assays were used as response variables. Using a repeated-measures anova, C. maenas consumption rates among the four size categories of L. littorea (within-subject treatments, fixed, four levels) were compared between early and late summer (random, two levels).

Feeding preference of C. maenas

To test whether C. maenas shows a preference between different types of mollusc prey, multi-choice feeding assays were conducted in the laboratory. On 6 September 2007, 20 aquaria were set up (specifications of the general set-up as above), containing each one blue mussel (M. edulis (L.)), one oyster (Crassostrea gigas Thunberg 1793), two L. Littorea (one specimen 18 to < 20 mm and the other 20 to < 23 mm), and one male C. maenas, starved for 2 days prior to assays. The identity and number of consumed prey items was recorded at the end of the 7-day-long experiment and used as the response variable that was analysed by resampling without replacement, using a Monte Carlo analysis with 10 000 permutations (Bärlocher 1999).

Non-consumptive indirect predator effects

Two laboratory and three field experiments were conducted, to assess the non-consumptive effects of C. maenas on L. littorea consumption rates. Littorina littorea food consisted of 6- to 8-week-old F. serratus recruits. To generate recruits, eggs were fertilized in the laboratory and zygotes seeded onto unglazed ceramic tiles (9.6 × 9.6 cm). After 6 to 8 weeks of incubation at 15 °C and 54 μmol photons s−1 m−2 in a 12:12 dark:light rhythm, zygotes developed into c. 2-mm-tall recruits.

At the beginning of the laboratory experiments, two ceramic tiles covered with F. serratus recruits were placed in each aquarium (specifications see above). Each aquarium was divided by a green plastic mesh (1 mm mesh size) into an up- and downstream compartment of equal size. In the upstream compartment, C. maenas could hide in an 8-cm-long PVC pipe (Ø 9 cm). The downstream compartment was further divided by an opaque PVC plate into two chambers of equal size, with each chamber containing one ceramic tile covered with F. serratus recruits (Fig. 1). Experiments were conducted in early and late summer from 19 to 27 June 2008 and 14 to 22 September 2008, respectively. Treatments included (i) controls, containing in both downstream chambers only tiles covered with F. serratus recruits (Fig. 1); (ii) L. littorea grazing aquaria, in which three small (<18 mm) and two larger (≥ 20 mm) periwinkles were each randomly allocated to one downstream chamber in addition to tiles covered with F. serratus recruits; (iii) predation risk aquaria, which were set up like L. littorea grazing aquaria but contained in addition a single male C. maenas (47.9 ± 8.6 mm) in the upstream compartment and (iv) C. maenas artefact aquaria, in which upstream compartments contained a single male C. maenas (48.6 ± 7.3 mm), while each downstream compartment contained one tile covered with F. serratus recruits but no L. littorea. Assuming that the soft body mass of periwinkles is a good proxy for grazing impact, L. littorea soft body mass was non-destructively calculated prior to experiments to ensure comparable L. littorea grazing impact in chambers containing small and large periwinkles. Adopting the methods described in Trussell (2000), we determined the soft body mass of 64 L. littorea between 12 and 28 cm shell length (four specimens per 1 mm interval) for converting shell length into soft body mass (linear regression: y = 0.2863x, = 64, r2 = 0.831). The soft body mass of small and large L. littorea in both chambers of each aquarium was a non-significant ≤ 1% different (Student’s t-test: early summer: t14 = 0.63, P = 0.536; late summer: t38 = 0.02, = 0.988). Consequently, we assumed identical overall consumption impact in chambers containing small or large L. littorea.

image

Figure 1.  Experimental set-up of laboratory experiments testing for non-consumptive predator effects. Arrow indicates direction of water flow. Stippled lines indicate mesh, dividing aquaria into up- and downstream compartments. Thick solid lines indicate opaque PVC plates separating downstream compartments into two chambers containing either small (<18 mm) or large (≥ 20 mm) Littorina littorea feeding on Fucus serratus recruits grown on ceramic tiles (grey quadrates). Thick-lined quadrates indicate tiles used for testing the effects of predation risk on intraspecific competition in L. littorea.

Download figure to PowerPoint

Seven shell-crushed L. littorea (< 18 mm) were added as C. maenas food to upstream compartments. Experiments in early and late summer comprised a total of 16 and 40 aquaria, respectively, which were grouped in quartets on separate tiers, and irradiated with light tubes at 34 μmol photons s−1 m−2. Each of the four treatments was present per quartet and was randomly positioned on each tier, resulting in a sample size of four and ten in the early and late summer experiment, respectively. Assays were terminated when F. serratus recruits of at least five tiles were grazed down to ≤ 10%. Differences in the consumption of F. serratus recruits by L. littorea of different size (within-subject factor, fixed, two levels) in the presence and absence of predation risk (grouping factor, fixed, two levels) were analysed with repeated-measures anova. Separate two-way anovas compared grazing impact of large and small L. littorea, respectively, with controls to assess main and interactive effects of predation risk and L. littorea grazing on survival of F. serratus recruits.

Three field experiments were conducted to assess whether the presence of nearby consuming C. maenas affected the grazing behaviour of L. littorea. Two field experiments were conducted from 16 to 24 July and 9 to 17 September 2008, in which the set-up from Trussell, Ewanchuk & Bertness (2002) was adopted. In both experiments, ten circular (Ø 1.2 m) plots were randomly positioned at a minimum distance of 6.5 m at Littorina Barren. Each plot consisted of nine perforated orange PVC pipes (Ø = 20 cm, 15 cm high) that were bolted evenly spaced in a circular pattern to the substratum at the perimeter of each plot. The open ends of each pipe ( = crab cage) were closed with a green plastic mesh of 0.1 cm mesh size. In the centre of each plot, four cubical cages ( = Littorina cages) made of green plastic mesh (mesh size 4 mm) were bolted to the substratum. On the day the experiments started ceramic tiles (9.6 × 9.6 cm) with F. serratus recruits were fixed inside each Littorina cage. Furthermore, two (early summer) or five (late summer) L. littorea (<18 mm) were randomly added to two of the four Littorina cages of each plot ( = grazing treatment), while no L. littorea were added to the remaining two Littorina cages of each plot ( = control). The number of L. littorina was increased in late summer to ensure that missing effects in early summer were not caused by low grazing pressure. Finally, one male C. maenas (carapace width >58 mm) together with seven shell-crushed L. littorea (<18 mm) for food were added to each crab cage of five randomly chosen plots (predation risk treatment), while seven shell-crushed L. littorea without C. maenas were added to crab cages of the remaining five plots (control). Five of the 90 C. maenas imprisoned in both experiments died (5.5%) but casualties never exceeded more than one individual per plot. Dead C. maenas were not replaced. The third field experiment used a different set-up and was conducted from 16 to 26 July 2009. Instead of using nine small crab cages in each plot, circular (Ø 1.7 m) plastic cages (4 mm mesh size) were bolted to the shore at Littorina Barren. Each of these 10 cages (= crab arena) was 30 cm high and closed at the bottom and top with the same plastic mesh. In the centre of each crab arena, four Littorina cages (specifications see above) were fixed with cable ties to the bottom mesh. Ten C. maenas were added to half of the crab arenas and were fed daily with 10 shell-crushed M. edulis. In addition, L. littorea were added at ambient density to crab arenas. Field experiments testing for the effects of predation risk (two levels, fixed) on periwinkle grazing (two levels, fixed) were analysed by three-way nested anovas, with plots nested within predation risk.

Assessment of L. littorina consumption

A 1-cm band along the margin of tiles was excluded from sampling to avoid edge effects. To determine the density of F. serratus recruits, all recruits from 21 randomly selected 1-cm2 sub-samples ( = 36% of the tile area that was sampled) were counted on each tile prior to and at the end of experiments, using a stereo microscope (12× magnification). The proportion of the number of remaining recruits was calculated and used as the response variable in the statistical analyses.

Statistical analysis

Prior to the analyses, proportional data were arcsin-transformed, normality of differences confirmed with Kolmogorov–Smirnov test (paired t-test), and homogeneous variances confirmed with Cochran’s test. Data showing heterogeneous variances were log-transformed to meet the assumptions. In case heterogeneous variances persisted after transformation, the level of significance was reduced to α = 0.01. The assumption of sphericity was tested for dependent data with Mauchly’s test if more than two within-subject treatments occurred (Quinn & Keough 2002). When the sphericity assumption was violated, F-tests were adjusted using the Greenhouse–Geisser estimate and adjusted degrees of freedom were reported. Post hoc comparisons were done with Tukey’s Honestly Significant Differences test.

Results

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials & methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References

Carcinus maenas activity and abundance of L. littorea

A total of 449 C. maenas with an average (± SD) carapace width of 48.3 (± 11.31) (males) and 40.6 (± 6.92) mm (females), were encountered at Littorina Barren. There were significantly 4.5 times more male than female C. maenas present at the study site (paired t-test: t35 = 9.51, = 0.000).

Up to 829 L. littorea specimens per square metre were collected at the study site. The total number of L. littorea per square metre was not significantly different between sampling events (two-way RM-anova: F4;55 = 2.28, = 0.072, Fig. 2). Yet, there were significant differences in the abundance of L. littorea among size classes (two-way RM-anova: F1.4;165 = 68.68, < 0.0001, Fig. 2). The smallest L. littorea were, on average, 49%, 72% and 38% less abundant than conspecifics of 17–20 mm, 20–23 mm and > 23 mm in shell height, respectively. Moreover, L. littorea of 20–23 mm in height were, on average, 1.8 and 2.2 times more abundant than conspecifics of 17–20 mm and > 23 mm, respectively, with a similar mean density of L. littorea between the latter two size classes (Fig. 2). Differences in the density of differently sized L. littorea were dependent on the time of sampling (two-way RM-anova: F5.6;165 = 12.75, < 0.0001). While L. littorea population structure followed a similar trend among seasons, magnitude of relative differences in abundance of periwinkles of different size classes varied seasonally. For instance, in both early (July) and late summer (September) of 2008, the density of the smallest L. littorea was, on average, eight and four times lower than the density of 20–23 mm and > 23 mm large conspecifics, respectively, while a similar density of L. littorea of some sizes was recorded in April 2008 (Fig. 2).

image

Figure 2.  Mean (+SEM) number of individual Littorina littorea per square metre of four different size classes at Littorina Barren between September 2007 and 2008. Stippled line = average density of L. littorea at NW Atlantic site of the Trussell, Ewanchuk & Bertness (2002) study. = 12.

Download figure to PowerPoint

Consumption rates by C. maenas

None of the female C. maenas consumed L. littorea, while 50% and 45% of the male C. maenas consumed L. littorea in early and late summer, respectively. Female C. maenas were a significant 18% smaller than males (Student’s t-test: t18 = 5.65, < 0.001), whereas consuming males were a non-significant 4% larger than non-consuming males (Student’s t-test: t16 = 1.13, = 0.243). Male C. maenas significantly preferred small (< 18 mm) to larger L. littorea, at a rate that was, on average, five times higher (two-way RM-anova: F3,48 = 20.32, < 0.0001, Fig. 3). This preference persisted during the study period, as indicated by a missing interaction between L. littorea size and season (two-way RM-anova: F3,48 = 1.37, = 0.263). Furthermore, feeding preferences of C. maenas were not different between seasons (two-way RM-anova: F1,16 = 0.03, = 0.878). With the exception of one assay, the two smallest L. littorea per EU were consumed prior to the larger conspecifics.

image

Figure 3.  Mean (+SEM) number of consumed Littorina littorea of different size categories by Carcinus maenas during 14-day-long feeding assays. Consumption rates on size categories sharing a letter were not significantly different. = 9 and 10 in early and late summer, respectively.

Download figure to PowerPoint

Feeding preference of C. maenas

Carcinus maenas preferred C. gigas and M. edulis significantly to L. littorea (resampling: < 0.0001). Ten C. maenas consumed both C. gigas and M. edulis, while five C. maenas consumed either M. edulis or C. gigas. None of the L. littorea were consumed.

Non-consumptive predator effects (laboratory)

In early summer, the presence of nearby consuming C. maenas was significantly beneficial for the density of L. littorea-exposed F. serratus recruits (two-way RM-anova: F1,6 = 179.88, < 0.001). Fifteen times more recruits remained on tiles in the presence of L. littorea in aquaria with than without C. maenas (Fig. 4a). The density of F. serratus recruits was neither significantly different between treatments using small and large L. littorea (F1,6 = 1.14, = 0.328), nor was the effect of predation risk dependent on the size of L. littorea (Size × Predation Risk interaction: F1,6 = 2.86, = 0.142). Using 2 two-way anovas, recruit survivorship of tiles exposed to both, small or large L. littorea, was dependent on the presence of C. maenas (Table 1). Regardless of the size of L. littorea, F. serratus recruit density was not significantly different between aquaria with and without L. littorea when C. maenas was present. However, when C. maenas was absent, significantly fewer recruits (i.e. 15 times) remained on tiles with small as well as with large L. littorea than on tiles without L. littorea (Fig. 4a).

image

Figure 4.  Laboratory assays. Mean (+SEM) percent of Fucus serratus recruits remaining on settlement tiles after 8-day-exposure to small (white bars), large (cross-hatched bars) or without (black bars) Littorina littorea in the presence and absence of non-lethal Carcinus maenas in (a) early and (b) late summer 2008. Treatments sharing a letter were not significantly different. Greek letters illustrate statistical results from RM-anovas between small and large L. littorea, while minor and capital letters illustrate results from two-way anovas comparing grazing effects of small and large L. littorea, respectively, with L. littorea-free control treatments (see Materials and methods for details). = 4 and 10 in early and late summer, respectively. X marks L. littorea-free control treatments, crab denotes treatments with predation risk.

Download figure to PowerPoint

Table 1.   Results of two-way anovas. Comparing the effects of grazing by (A) small and (B) large periwinkles (Littorina littorea) to grazer-free controls on the density of Fucus serratus recruits in early (= 4) and late (= 10) summer in the presence and absence of risk cues of predatory crabs (Carcinus maenas) during 8-day-long laboratory assays. d.f., degrees of freedom; MS, mean square; significant results in bold
 Early summerLate summer
Sourced.f.MSFPd.f.MSFP
A
 Small Littorina (L)1383551.01<0.001125069.57<0.001
 Risk cue (R)1173223.03<0.0011584622.31<0.001
 L × R1296739.46<0.0011572921.86<0.001
 Residual1275  36262  
B
 Large Littorina (L)14132108.7<0.00114761.690.202
 Risk cue (R)1176446.38<0.001116005.670.023
 L × R1300979.11<0.001115395.450.025
 Residual1238  36282  

In late summer, L. littorea grazing was significantly affected by the presence of nearby consuming C. maenas (two-way RM-anova: F1,18 = 40.54, < 0.001). The density of F. serratus recruits in the presence of L. littorea was, on average, 1.8 times higher in aquaria with than without C. maenas (Fig. 4b). Recruit density was not significantly different between treatments using small and large L. littorea (two-way RM-anova: F1,18 = 2.29, = 0.148). Furthermore, a non-significant interaction between predation risk and L. littorea size indicates that the presence of nearby consuming C. maenas had a similar effect on the grazing impact of large and small L. littorea (two-way RM-anova: F1,18 = 3.81, = 0.067). Two 2-way anovas revealed that survival of F. serratus recruits exposed to either small or large L. littorea was a significant 2.3 and 1.4 times, respectively, lower on tiles with than without L. littorea, but only in aquaria where C. maenas was absent from the upstream compartment as indicated by the significant Littorina × Risk cue interaction (Table 1, Fig. 4b).

Non-consumptive predator effects (field)

Littorina littorea grazing significantly reduced the density of F. serratus recruits in early and late summer 2008 and summer 2009 by, on average, 26%, 45% and 50%, respectively (Fig. 5). This effect was not changed by the presence of nearby consuming C. maenas in early and late summer 2008 (Table 2). Using the different experimental set-up in 2009; however, the effect of L. littorea grazing on recruit density of F. serratus was dependent on the presence of C. maenas as indicated by a significant L. littorea × C. maenas interaction (Table 2). In the absence of C. maenas, L. littorea reduced the recruit density of F. serratus by a significant 65%, whereas the grazing impact of L. littorea was significantly weaker (about 33% reduction in recruit density), albeit significant, in the presence of nearby consuming C. maenas (Fig. 5).

image

Figure 5.  Field assays. Mean (+SEM) percentage of Fucus serratus recruits remaining on settlement tiles after 8 (a, b) or 10 days (c) with or without Littorina littorea in the presence and absence of nearby consuming Carcinus maenas. Treatments sharing a letter were not significantly different. = 5, grey background marking = experiment using crab arenas, crab denotes treatments with predation risk.

Download figure to PowerPoint

Table 2.   Field experiments. Results of nested three-way anovas comparing the effects of Littorina littorea grazing on the number of Fucus serratus-recruits in the presence and absence of risk cues of the predator Carcinus maenas. Elimination of error term L × Plot(Risk cue) and recalculation of residuals was done for the 2009 experiment only, after verifying that σ2 L × Plot(Risk cue) = 0, i.e. not significant at α = 0.25, with used denominator mean square (MQden) for each source of variation shown in column MQden. Pooled term = L × Plot(Risk cue) + Residual, d.f.pooled, degrees of freedom after elimination of error term L × Plot(Risk cue), significant results at α = 0.05 in bold, = 5
SourceEarly summer 2008Late summer 2008Early summer 2009MQden
d.f.MQFPMQFPd.f.pooledMQFP
Risk cue (R)1543.73.160.09126.580.0870.7721210310.610.012Plot(Risk cue)
Littorina (L)1240814.010.001847427.62<0.0011554950.13<0.001pooled
R × L1214.01.250.27832.420.110.7491141512.790.001pooled
Plot(Risk Cue)8292.41.700.160513.91.680.16681981.790.122pooled
L × Plot(Risk cue)8258.01.500.219571.51.860.124 1050.930.517Residual
Residual20171.9  306.8  28113   
Pooled        110   

Discussion

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials & methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References

Carcinus maenas preferred small over larger L. littorea in early and in late summer. Furthermore, the presence of nearby consuming C. maenas altered at both times the foraging behaviour of L. littorea under laboratory conditions, but did so in the field only when large crab exclosures were used. Despite the observed size-dependent risk of consumption by C. maenas, the level of intimidation was similar in small and large L. littorea in laboratory experiments.

Several findings of this study intuitively suggest that consumptive effects of C. maenas on the L. littorea population and thus beneficial indirect effects on recruit survival of F. serratus should be of limited impact. First, the consumption of periwinkles by C. maenas was dependent on the size of snails. Small periwinkles (< 18 mm) were more often consumed by green crabs than larger conspecifics. This size-specific preference was apparent in laboratory experiments conducted in early and in late summer. As 80–95% of the periwinkles encountered at the study site were larger than the preferred size class, the majority of the adult L. littorea population dwell under a relatively low C. maenas predation pressure. Studies by Buschbaum et al. (2007) and Perez et al. (2009) corroborate patterns in size-specific consumption of L. littorina by green crabs on NE and NW Atlantic shores. Like other molluscs, larger periwinkles may lower or even escape predation by shell-thickening (e.g. Freeman & Byers 2006). Hereby, infestation by shell-boring organisms like the polychaete Polydora ciliata can modify shell strength in L. littorea, reducing the size refuge from crab predation for periwinkles (Buschbaum et al. 2007). Yet, the Helgoland population of L. littorea appeared hardly infested by P. ciliata or trematodes (Eschweiler, Molis & Buschbaum 2009), indicating that larger L. littorea should be relatively safe from green crab predation at our study site. Second, only males of the C. maenas population on Helgoland consumed L. littorea. This can be due to sexual dimorphism in claw morphology and possibly the observed smaller size of females to males, favouring the latter in crushing mollusc prey (Elner 1980). According to trap samples, at least 22% of C. maenas individuals were females so that L. littorina will be consumed by a maximum of 78% of the individuals that constitute the C. maenas population at the study site. Third, only half of the male C. maenas consumed periwinkles during 14-day-long assays. A similar size of consuming and non-consuming C. maenas suggests that other factors than morphological features could explain this pattern. This finding, in combination with the observed strong rejection of periwinkles by C. maenas when alternative food is present, suggests that L. littorea is an unattractive diet for C. maenas. Nevertheless, occasional presence of cracked L. littorea shells at the study site (M. Molis, pers. obs.) indicates that some periwinkle consumption by shell-crushing predators like C. maenas takes place.

In the light of this relatively low predation pressure by C. maenas on L. littorea, the strong reduction in food intake by periwinkles in the presence of nearby consuming C. maenas in laboratory assays is striking. Besides a 15-fold higher chance for F. serratus recruits to survive L. littorea grazing in the presence than in the absence of C. maenas, it is even more noticeable that the number of survivors was almost identical between treatments with and without L. littorea when crabs were present. This indicates that nearby consuming C. maenas induced such a high level of intimidation that grazing by L. littorea was almost completely ‘switched off’. This strong positive indirect effect of predation risk on F. serratus recruit survival corroborates results of the field study by Trussell, Ewanchuk & Bertness (2002). Thus, non-consumptive predator effects can be beneficial for the survival of basal species (e.g. Coleman et al. 2007), affect energy transfer (Trussell, Ewanchuk & Matassa 2006a) and probably ultimately species composition and functioning of communities (Schmitz et al. 2008). However, our study also indicates that the strength of risk effects can vary temporally. Intimidation of periwinkles was at its maximum when F. serratus ceased reproduction (July–August) and at its minimum during the main reproductive period of F. serratus, i.e. from September to May (Kornmann & Sahling 1993), which is also the period when C. maenas overwinters offshore. A strong predation risk effect in early summer should maximize survival of small recruits that settled during spring, while the higher recruitment of F. serratus, taking place at a time when the indirect protection by C. maenas from periwinkle grazing will be less strong, i.e. in late summer, may compensate grazing-induced recruitment losses.

Our study revealed that survivorship of F. serratus recruits was similar between small and larger periwinkles in the presence of nearby consuming C. maenas. In contrast to the findings by Vadas, Burrows & Hughes (1994) using dogwhelks (N. lapillus), our finding indicates that for L. littorea the level of intimidation was independent of body size. Therefore, non-consumptive predator effects seem to have no effect on intraspecific competition for food in L. littorea. According to the high predation rate of small periwinkles by C. maenas, we expected small L. littorea to be significantly more intimidated by predation risk than larger specimens and, thus, survival of F. serratus recruits in the presence of nearby consuming C. maenas to be significantly lower when exposed to larger than to small periwinkles. Nevertheless, the lack of such patterns provides evidence of the importance of non-consumptive relative to consumptive predator effects in this simple food chain. Due to consumptive predator effects, an enhancement of the recruitment success of F. serratus is limited to the reduction of the grazing impact by small periwinkles (< 18 mm), which crabs preferably remove from the population. In contrast, predation risk reduced the grazing impact of small and larger periwinkles regardless of real predation threat equally strong, suggesting that non-consumptive predator effects will lower the grazing impact of all periwinkles. Therefore, recruitment success of F. serratus should be much higher due to non-consumptive rather than consumptive predator effects. Thus, periwinkle food, like seaweeds, should benefit more from the intimidation than the consumption of L. littorea by C. maenas. Although we did not directly test this, this statement is corroborated by experimental evidence from other studies on the contribution of risk effects to the net effect of predation. These studies suggest that non-consumptive predator effects can be at least as strong as the consumptive effects of a predator (Peacor & Werner 2001; Trussell, Ewanchuk & Matassa 2006b). Moreover, our study shows that even relatively unattractive prey responds strongly to predation risk, suggesting that non-consumptive predator effects may affect a relatively broad range of species and not only preferred prey, while consumptive predator effects should affect attractive prey disproportionately stronger. This indicates that non-consumptive predator effects may increase the connectivity among species more than consumptive effects do, with possible positive effects on ecosystem function (Schmitz et al. 2008) and diversity (Schmitz 2006).

Only our field experiments using large crab exclosures (2009 experiment) corroborate beneficial effects of predation risk on survival of F. serratus recruits observed in laboratory assays. This effect was mainly caused by a stronger reduction in recruit density from grazing L. littorea in the absence of C. maenas when crab exclosures instead of open plots (used in 2008 experiments) were used. Perhaps, open plots were trespassed by wild C. maenas thereby intimidating L. littorina in control plots equally strong to conspecifics inside risk plots. Differences between Trussell, Ewanchuk & Bertness (2002) and our 2008 experiments in L. littorea grazing impact in the absence of risk cues may be due to a lower activity level of C. maenas at the former than at our study site. Trussell, Ewanchuk & Bertness (2002) did not report on the activity or abundance of C. maenas at their study site. Our data from trap samples suggest that twelve C. maenas trespass on average daily through a plot. Although it is unknown whether this exposure level will sufficiently affect periwinkle foraging behaviour, models by Lima & Bednekoff (1999) suggest that irregular exposure to predator cues could elicit a stronger anti-predator behaviour than a constant level of exposure.

In contrast to our laboratory experiments and the field study by Trussell, Ewanchuk & Bertness (2002), recruit density was in all three field experiments more strongly reduced by L. littorea grazing in the presence of C. maenas than in risk plots where L. littorea was absent. This suggests that non-consumptive effects among C. maenas, L. littorea and F. serratus are not general and that perhaps some environmental factor(s) exist that mediate the strength of predation risk differently among the study sites used by Trussell, Ewanchuk & Bertness (2002) and our study. At least four factors seem likely to explain site-specific differences in trait-mediated indirect interactions. First, differences in wave exposure may dilute the concentration of risk cues below a threshold concentration that L. littorea may require to be intimidated (Ferrari, Wisenden & Chivers 2010). The site where Trussell, Ewanchuk & Bertness (2002) conducted their study was sheltered (S. Hawkins, pers. comm.). This could explain a stronger effect size of predation risk there compared to our study, which was conducted at a shore of intermediate wave-exposure (M. Molis, unpubl. data). Similarly, the distance between C. maenas and prey was larger in the field than in the laboratory where the strength of predation risk effects was higher, fostering the possibility that cue dilution may lower strength of risk effects. The study by Freeman & Hamer (2009) corroborates this possibility as the effect of crab predation risk on dogwhelk foraging behaviour was lower in wave-exposed than in sheltered environments, suggesting that wave-exposure history can drive the effect size of trait-mediated indirect interactions. Second, the effects of predation risk may depend on prey density (Ferrari, Wisenden & Chivers 2010). The level of intimidation in L. littorea may perhaps decline with increasing periwinkle density because the individual risk of falling prey to a C. maenas should decrease for L. littorea as the number of conspecifics increases. The study by Edwards & Davies (2002) suggests that mucus trails of L. littorea contain some unknown cue(s). Trail-bound cues may provide information for a L. littorea specimen about the density of conspecifics. At the study site of Trussell, Ewanchuk & Bertness (2002), where crab predation risk influenced periwinkle foraging behaviour, periwinkle density was, on average, about seven times lower than at our study site, which may explain strong predation risk effects at their study site and weaker effects in our study. Third, differences in habitat heterogeneity may alter encounter rates between predators and prey, thereby changing predation risk. For instance, Lewis & Eby (2002) showed that predation risk for snails was reduced in intertidal saltmarsh habitats with complex structure. Furthermore, habitat heterogeneity and strength of predation risk can be interdependent in food chains where resources affect habitat structure. Trussell, Ewanchuk & Matassa (2008) demonstrated that the identity of resources can influence the degree of risk perceived by prey. By creating complex habitats, blue mussels increase refuge availability for dogwhelks from mollusc-eating crabs, by which dogwhelks experience a reduced predation risk. Fourth, the magnitude of predation risk effects may depend on resource availability (Preisser, Bolnick & Grabowski 2009). According to their meta-analysis, the non-consumptive effects of C. maenas should be stronger in habitats where changes in resources like seaweed recruits will vary dynamically in response to grazing by L. littorea than in habitats where resource availability would be independent of grazing. As seaweed recruits were not renewed in our study, the effect of predation risk should have been, with respect to a potential influence of resource dynamics, at least as strong as in the Trussell, Ewanchuk & Bertness (2002) study.

Despite short assay times, our study indicates for at least two reasons that F. serratus recruitment may benefit in the longer term from risk effects of nearby consuming C. maenas. First, risk cues have the potential to almost completely ‘switch off’ periwinkle consumption as indicated by the almost identical survivorship of F. serratus recruits in the presence and absence of periwinkles when nearby crabs consumed conspecific snails. Second, despite differences in the threat of real predation, risk effects showed no intraspecific variation. Thus, our study provides additional evidence about the causes by which non-consumptive predator effects can affect prey performance and indirectly survivorship of basal species to a greater extent than consumptive predator effects. Such examples on positive trait-mediated indirect effects on recruit survival of resources indicate that the persistence of basal species, like plants and seaweeds, may be to a larger extend top-down controlled than previously anticipated (sensuPeckarsky et al. 2008). The accumulating evidence that non-consumptive predator effects surmount their consumptive effects (e.g. Peacor & Werner 2001; Trussell, Ewanchuk & Matassa 2006b) provides insight into ecological mechanisms, e.g. on recruitment success of basal species under a high density of consumers. Thus, non-consumptive predator effects can facilitate the establishment and/or persistence of seaweed populations in hostile environments. Knowledge on facilitation in ecological processes is important for a comprehensive understanding for ecosystem functioning and structure (e.g. Bruno, Stachowicz & Bertness 2003). One ultimate consequence of positive indirect interactions between predators and resources are changes in the evenness and thus diversity and structure of communities as the number of survivors will increase in communities in which important consumers are affected by predation risk (Schmitz 2006). Consequently, our study suggests that interaction webs may provide more information than food webs about drivers of structure, function and perhaps stability of ecological communities.

Acknowledgements

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials & methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References

We thank the crew of RV Aade for collection of green crabs and help in setting up the experiment in the field. Field assistance by K. Metzger and Tobias Vonnahme is greatly acknowledged, as is help with the maintenance of green crabs by M. Krüss and staff of the Aquarium Helgoland. A. Wagner is thanked for technical support. We are grateful to C. Buschbaum for animal drawings.

References

  1. Top of page
  2. Summary
  3. Introduction
  4. Materials & methods
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. References
  • Bärlocher, F. (1999) Biostatistics. Thieme Verlag, Stuttgart.
  • Bartsch, I. & Tittley, I. (2004) The rocky intertidal biotopes of Helgoland: present and past. Helgoland Marine Research, 58, 289302.
  • Bruno, J.F., Stachowicz, J.J. & Bertness, M.D. (2003) Inclusion of facilitation into ecological theory. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 18, 119125.
  • Buschbaum, C., Buschbaum, G., Schrey, I. & Thieltges, D.W. (2007) Shell-boring polychaetes affect gastropod shell strength and crab predation. Marine Ecology-Progress Series, 329, 123130.
  • Coleman, R.A., Ramchunder, S.J., Davies, K.M., Moody, A.J. & Foggo, A. (2007) Herbivore-induced infochemicals influence foraging behaviour in two intertidal predators. Oecologia, 151, 454463.
  • Creel, S. & Christianson, D. (2008) Relationship between direct predation and risk effects. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 23, 194201.
  • Edwards, M. & Davies, M.S. (2002) Functional and ecological aspects of the mucus trails of the intertidal prosobranch gastropod Littorina littorea. Marine Ecology-Progress Series, 239, 129137.
  • Elner, R.W. (1980) The influence of temperature, sex and chela size in the foraging strategy of the shore crab, Carcinus maenas (L). Marine Behaviour and Physiology, 7, 1524.
  • Enderlein, P., Moorthi, S., Rohrscheidt, H. & Wahl, M. (2003) Optimal foraging versus shared doom effects: interactive influence of mussel size and epibiosis on predator preference. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 292, 231242.
  • Eschweiler, N., Molis, M. & Buschbaum, C. (2009) Habitat-specific size structure variations in periwinkle populations (Littorina littorea) caused by biotic factors. Helgoland Marine Research, 63, 119127.
  • Ferrari, M.C.O., Wisenden, B.D. & Chivers, D.P. (2010) Chemical ecology of predator–prey interactions in aquatic ecosystems: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 88, 698724.
  • Fine, M. & Loya, Y. (2003) Alternate coral-bryozoan competitive superiority during coral bleaching. Marine Biology, 142, 989996.
  • Flynn, A.M. & Smee, D.L. (2010) Behavioral plasticity of the soft-shell clam, Mya arenaria (L.), in the presence of predators increases survival in the field. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 383, 3238.
  • Freeman, A.S. & Byers, J.E. (2006) Divergent induced responses to an invasive predator in marine mussel populations. Science, 313, 831833.
  • Freeman, A.S. & Hamer, C.E. (2009) The persistent effect of wave exposure on TMIIs and crab predation in Nucella lapillus. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 372, 5863.
  • Hay, M.E., Parker, J.D., Burkepile, D.E., Caudill, C.C., Wilson, A.E., Hallinan, Z.P. & Chequer, A.D. (2004) Mutualisms and aquatic community structure: the enemy of my enemy is my friend. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 35, 175197.
  • Jacobsen, H.P. & Stabell, O.B. (1999) Predator-induced alarm responses in the common periwinkle, Littorina littorea: dependence on season, light conditions, and chemical labelling of predators. Marine Biology, 134, 551557.
  • Janke, K. (1986) Die Makrofauna und ihre Verteilung im Nordost-Felswatt von Helgoland. Helgoländer Meeresuntersuchungen, 40, 155.
  • Kauffman, M.J., Brodie, J.F. & Jules, E.S. (2010) Are wolves saving Yellowstone’s aspen? A landscape-level test of a behaviorally mediated trophic cascade. Ecology, 91, 27422755.
  • Koch, N., Lynch, B. & Rochette, R. (2007) Trade-off between mating and predation risk in the marine snail, Littorina plena. Invertebrate Biology, 126, 257267.
  • Kornmann, P. & Sahling, P.-H. (1993) Meeresalgen von Helgoland - benthische Grün-, Braun- und, Rotalgen. Biologische Anstalt Helgoland, Hamburg.
  • Lewis, D.B. & Eby, L.A. (2002) Spatially heterogeneous refugia and predation risk in intertidal salt marshes. Oikos, 96, 119129.
  • Lima, S.L. & Bednekoff, P.A. (1999) Temporal variation in danger drives antipredator behavior: the predation risk allocation hypothesis. The American Naturalist, 153, 649659.
  • Long, J.D. & Trussell, G.C. (2007) Geographic variation in seaweed induced responses to herbivory. Marine Ecology-Progress Series, 333, 7580.
  • Lubchenco, J. (1978) Plant species diversity in a marine intertidal community: importance of herbivore food preference on algal competitive abilities. American Naturalist, 112, 2329.
  • Ohgushi, T. (2005) Indirect interaction webs: herbivore-induced effects through trait change in plants. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 36, 81105.
  • Peacor, S.D. & Werner, E.E. (2000) Predator effects on an assemblage of consumers through induced changes in consumer foraging behavior. Ecology, 81, 19982010.
  • Peacor, S.D. & Werner, E.E. (2001) The contribution of trait-mediated indirect effects to the net effects of a predator. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 39043908.
  • Peckarsky, B.L., Abrams, P.A., Bolnick, D.I., Dill, L.M., Grabowski, J.H., Luttbeg, B., Orrock, J.L., Peacor, S.D., Preisser, E.L., Schmitz, O.J. & Trussell, G.C. (2008) Revisiting the classics: considering nonconsumptive effects in textbook examples of predator–prey interactions. Ecology, 89, 24162425.
  • Perez, K.O., Carlson, R.L., Shulman, M.J. & Ellis, J.C. (2009) Why are intertidal snails rare in the subtidal? Predation, growth and the vertical distribution of Littorina littorea (L.) in the Gulf of Maine. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 369, 7986.
  • Petraitis, P.S. (2002) Effects of intraspecific competition and scavenging on growth of the periwinkle Littorina littorea. Marine Ecology-Progress Series, 236, 179187.
  • Preisser, E.L., Bolnick, D.I. & Benard, M.F. (2005) Scared to death? The effects of intimidation and consumption in predator–prey interactions. Ecology, 86, 501509.
  • Preisser, E.L., Bolnick, D.I. & Grabowski, J.H. (2009) Resource dynamics influence the strength of non-consumptive predator effects on prey. Ecology Letters, 12, 315323.
  • Quinn, G.P. & Keough, M.J. (2002) Experimental Design and Data Analysis for Biologists. University Press, Cambridge.
  • Reichert, K., Buchholz, F., Bartsch, I., Kersten, T. & Gimenez, L. (2008) Scale-dependent patterns of variability in species assemblages of the rocky intertidal at Helgoland (German Bight, North Sea). Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom, 88, 13191329.
  • Rochette, R. & Dill, L.M. (2000) Mortality, behavior and the effects of predators on the intertidal distribution of littorinid gastropods. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 253, 165191.
  • Schmitz, O. (2006) Predators have large effects on ecosystem properties by changing plant diversity, not plant biomass. Ecology, 87, 14321437.
  • Schmitz, O.J., Grabowski, J.H., Peckarsky, B.L., Preisser, E.L., Trussell, G.C. & Vonesh, J.R. (2008) From individuals to ecosystem function: toward an integration of evolutionary and ecosystem ecology. Ecology, 89, 24362445.
  • Simenstad, C.A., Estes, J.A. & Kenyon, K.W. (1978) Aleuts, sea otters, and alternate stable-state communities. Science, 200, 403411.
  • Spooner, E.H., Coleman, R.A. & Attrill, M.J. (2007) Sex differences in body morphology and multitrophic interactions involving the foraging behaviour of the crab Carcinus maenas. Marine Ecology-an Evolutionary Perspective, 28, 394403.
  • Trussell, G.C. (2000) Predator-induced plasticity and morphological trade-offs in latitudinally separated populations of Littorina obtusata. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2, 803822.
  • Trussell, G.C., Ewanchuk, P.J. & Bertness, M.D. (2002) Field evidence of trait-mediated indirect interactions in a rocky intertidal food web. Ecology Letters, 5, 241245.
  • Trussell, G.C., Ewanchuk, P.J. & Matassa, C.M. (2006a) The fear of being eaten reduces energy transfer in a simple food chain. Ecology, 87, 29792984.
  • Trussell, G.C., Ewanchuk, P.J. & Matassa, C.M. (2006b) Habitat effects on the relative importance of trait- and density-mediated indirect interactions. Ecology Letters, 9, 12451252.
  • Trussell, G.C., Ewanchuk, P.J. & Matassa, C.M. (2008) Resource identity modifies the influence of predation risk on ecosystem function. Ecology, 89, 27982807.
  • Vadas, R.L., Burrows, M.T. & Hughes, R.N. (1994) Foraging strategies of dogwhelks, Nucella lapillus (L.): interacting effects of age, diet and chemical cues to the threat of predation. Oecologia, 100, 439450.
  • Werner, E.E. & Peacor, S.D. (2003) A review of trait-mediated indirect interactions in ecological communities. Ecology, 84, 10831100.
  • Wootton, J.T. (2002) Indirect effects in complex ecosystems: recent progress and future challenges. Journal of Sea Research, 48, 157172.
  • Yun, H.Y., Cruz, J., Treitschke, M., Wahl, M. & Molis, M. (2007) Testing for the induction of anti-herbivory defences in four Portuguese macroalgae by direct and water-borne cues of grazing amphipods. Helgoland Marine Research, 61, 203209.