SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Abutalebi, J., Cappa, S.F., & Perani, D. (2001). The bilingual brain as revealed by functional neuroimaging. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4 (2), 179190.
  • Ardal, S., Donald, M.W., Meuter, R., Muldrew, S., & Luce, M. (1990). Brain responses to semantic incongruity in bilinguals. Brain and Language, 39, 187205.
  • Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70 (3), 636644.
  • Conboy, B.T. (2002). Patterns of language processing and growth in early English–Spanish bilingualism (Doctoral dissertation, University of California San Diego and San Diego State University, 2002). Dissertation Abstracts International, B: The Sciences & Engineering, 63(11-B), (UMI No. 5193).
  • Courchesne, E. (1978). Neurophysiological correlates of cognitive development: changes in long-latency event-related potentials from childhood to adulthood. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 45, 468482.
  • Döpke, S. (2000). Generation of and retraction from cross-linguistically motivated structures in bilingual first language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3 (3), 209226.
  • Fenson, L., Dale, P.S., Reznick, J.S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J.P., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J.S. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.
  • Gawlitzek-Maiwald, I., & Tracy, R. (1996). Bilingual bootstrapping. Linguistics, 34, 901926.
  • Grosjean, F. (2001). The bilingual's language modes. In J.L.Nicol (Ed.), One mind, two languages: Bilingual language processing (pp. 122). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
  • Hernández, A.E., Dapretto, M., Mazziotta, J., & Bookheimer, S. (2001). Language switching and language representation in Spanish–English bilinguals: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 14, 510520.
  • Hernández, A., Martínez, A., & Kohnert, K. (2000). In search of the language switch: an fMRI study of picture naming in Spanish–English bilinguals. Brain and Language, 73 (3), 421431.
  • Jackson, G.M., Swainson, R., Cunnington, R., & Jackson, S.R. (2001). ERP correlates of executive control during repeated language switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4 (2), 169178.
  • Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Marchman, V., Bates, E., & Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. (1993). Early lexical development of Spanish-speaking infants and toddlers. Journal of Child Language, 20, 523549.
  • Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Marchman, V., Fenson, L., Newton, T., & Conboy, B. (2003). El Inventario del Desarrollo de Habilidades Comunicativas: User's guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.
  • Karrer, R., & Ackles, P. (1987). Visual event-related potentials of infants during a modified oddball procedure. Current Trends in Event-Related Potential Research (EEG supplement), 40, 603608.
  • Marchman, V. (1999). Scoring program for the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories (Version 4.1) (Computer software). Retrieved from http://www.utdallas.edu/~vamarch/cdi/
  • Meisel, J.M. (2001). The simultaneous acquisition of two first languages: early differentiation and subsequent development of grammars. In J.Cenoz & F.Genesee (Eds.), Trends in bilingual acquisition (pp. 1142). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing Co.
  • Mills, D., Coffey-Corina, S., & Neville, H. (1993). Language acquisition and cerebral specialization in 20-month-old infants. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 317334.
  • Mills, D., Coffey-Corina, S., & Neville, H. (1997). Language comprehension and cerebral specialization from 13 to 20 months. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13 (3), 397445.
  • Mills, D., Conboy, B.T., & Paton, C. (2005). How learning new words shapes the organization of the infant brain. In L.Namy (Ed.), Symbol use and symbolic representation (pp. 123153). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Mills, D.L., & Neville, H. (1997). Electrophysiological studies of language and language impairment. In I.Rappin & R.Nass (Eds.), Special issue: Language disorders in children. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 4, 125134.
  • Mills, D., Plunkett, K., Prat, C., & Schafer, G. (2005). Watching the infant brain learn words: effects of language and experience. Cognitive Development, 20, 1931.
  • Mills, D., Prat, C., Stager, C., Zangl, R., Neville, H., & Werker, J. (2004). Language experience and the organization of brain activity to phonetically similar words: ERP evidence from 14- and 20-month-olds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 14521464.
  • Moreno, E., Federmeier, K., & Kutas, M. (2002). Switching languages, switching palabras (words): an electrophysiological study of code switching. Brain and Language, 80 (2), 188207.
  • Moreno, E., & Kutas, M. (2005). Processing semantic anomalies in two languages: an electrophysiological exploration in both languages of Spanish–English bilinguals. Cognitive Brain Research, 22 (2), 205220.
  • Müller, N. (1998). Transfer in bilingual first language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1 (3), 189192.
  • Nelson, C.A. (1994). Neural correlates of recognition memory in the first postnatal year. In G.Dawson & K.W.Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain (pp. 269313). New York: Guilford Publications.
  • Neville, H.J., Coffey, S.A., Lawson, D.S., Fischer, A., Emmorey, K., & Bellugi, U. (1997). Neural systems mediating American Sign Language: effects of sensory experience and age of acquisition. Brain and Language, 57, 285308.
  • Neville, H.J., Mills, D.L., & Lawson, D.S. (1992). Fractionating language: different neural subsystems with different sensitive periods. Cerebral Cortex, 2, 244258.
  • Paradis, M. (1990). Language lateralization in bilinguals: enough already! Brain and Language, 39, 576586.
  • Pearson, B.Z., Fernández, S., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D.K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual toddlers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 4158.
  • Pearson, B.Z., Fernández, S., & Oller, D.K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43, 43120.
  • Perani, D., Abutalebi, J., Paulesu, E., Brambati, P.S., Cappa, S.F., & Fazio, F. (2003). The role of age of acquisition and language usage in early, high-proficient bilinguals: an fMRI study during verbal fluency. Human Brain Mapping, 19, 170182.
  • Picton, T.W., Bentin, S., Berg, P., Donchin, E., Hillyard, S.A., Johnson, R., Miller, G.A., Ritter, W., Ruchkin, D.S, Rugg, M.D., & Taylor, M.J. (2000). Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. Psychophysiology, 37, 127152.
  • Price, C.J., Green, D.W., & von Studnitz, R. (1999). A functional imaging study of translation and language switching. Brain, 122, 22212235.
  • Vaid, J., & Hall, D.G. (1991). Neuropsychological perspectives on bilingualism: right, left, and center. In A.Reynolds (Ed.), Bilingualism, multiculturalism and second language learning (pp. 81112). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Weber-Fox, C.M., & Neville, H.J. (1996). Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8 (3), 231256.
  • Weber-Fox, C.M., & Neville, H.J. (2001). Sensitive periods differentiate processing of open- and closed-class words: an ERP study of bilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44 (6), 13381353.