SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Aguiar, A., & Baillargeon, R. (1999). 2.5-month-old infants’ reasoning about when objects should and should not be occluded. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 116157.
  • Albert, M. V., Schnabel, A., & Field, D. J. (2008). Innate visual learning through spontaneous activity patterns. PLoS Computational Biology, 4, 18.
  • Amso, D., & Johnson, S. P. (2006). Learning by selection: Visual search and object perception in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 6, 12361245.
  • Atkinson, J. (2000). The developing visual brain. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Baillargeon, R. (1994). How do infants learn about the physical world? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 133140.
    Direct Link:
  • Baillargeon, R. (2008). Innate ideas revisited: For a principle of persistence in infants’ physical reasoning. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 213.
    Direct Link:
  • Bednar, J. A., & Miikkulainen, R. (2007). Constructing visual function through prenatal and postnatal learning. In D.Mareschal, S.Sirois, G.Westermann, & M. H.Johnson (Eds.), Neuroconstructivism: Perspectives and prospects, Vol. 2. (pp. 1337). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Berger, A., Tzur, G., & Posner, M. L. (2006). Infant brains detect arithmetic errors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 103, 1264912653.
  • Bremner, J. G., Johnson, S. P., Slater, A., Mason, U., Cheshire, A., & Spring, J. (2007). Conditions for young infants’ failure to perceive trajectory continuity. Developmental Science, 10, 613624.
  • Bremner, J. G., Johnson, S. P., Slater, A. M., Mason, U., Foster, K., Cheshire, A., & Spring, J. (2005). Conditions for young infants’ perception of object trajectories. Child Development, 74, 10291043.
  • Bullock, D., Grossberg, S., & Guenther, F. H. (1993). A self-organizing neural model of motor equivalent reaching and tool use by a multijoint arm. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 408435.
  • Burkhalter, A. (1991). Developmental status of intrinsic connections in visual cortex of newborn human. In P.Bagnoli & W.Hodos (Eds.), The changing visual system (pp. 247254). New York: Plenum Press.
  • Burkhalter, A., Bernardo, K. L., & Charles, V. (1993). Development of local circuits in human visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 19161931.
  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Clifton, R. K., Rochat, P., Litovsky, R. Y., & Perris, E. E. (1991). Object representation guides infants’ reaching in the dark. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 323329.
  • Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1950). The perception of the visual world. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
  • Goldin-Meadow, S., & Mylander, C. (1998). Spontaneous sign systems created by deaf children in two cultures. Nature, 391, 279281.
  • Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Development of cortical circuitry and cognitive function. Child Development, 58, 601622.
  • Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1996). The prefrontal landscape: Implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 351, 14451453.
  • Gottlieb, J. P., Kusunoki, M., & Goldberg, M. E. (1998). The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex. Nature, 391, 481484.
  • Graham, D. J., & Field, D. J. (2007). Statistical regularities of art images and natural scenes: Spectra, sparseness and nonlinearities. Spatial Vision, 21, 149164.
  • Haith, M. M. (1998). Who put the cog in infant cognition? Is rich interpretation too costly? Infant Behavior & Development, 21, 167179.
  • Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research, 40, 14891506.
  • Johnson, S. P. (1997). Young infants’ perception of object unity: Implications for development of attentional and cognitive skills. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6, 511.
    Direct Link:
  • Johnson, S. P. (2004). Development of perceptual completion in infancy. Psychological Science, 15, 769775.
    Direct Link:
  • Johnson, S. P. (Ed.) (2010). Neoconstructivism: The new science of cognitive development. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Johnson, S. P., Amso, D., & Slemmer, J. A. (2003). Development of object concepts in infancy: Evidence for early learning in an eye tracking paradigm. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 100, 1056810573.
  • Johnson, S. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1995). Perception of object unity in 2-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 31, 739745.
  • Johnson, S. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1996). Perception of object unity in young infants: The roles of motion, depth, and orientation. Cognitive Development, 11, 161180.
  • Johnson, S. P., Bremner, J. G., Slater, A., Mason, U., Foster, K., & Cheshire, A. (2003). Infants’ perception of object trajectories. Child Development, 74, 94108.
  • Johnson, S. P., Davidow, J., Hall-Haro, C., & Frank, M. C. (2008). Development of perceptual completion originates in information acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 44, 12141224.
  • Johnson, S. P., & Johnson, K. L. (2000). Early perception-action coupling: Eye movements and the development of object perception. Infant Behavior & Development, 23, 461483.
  • Johnson, S. P., & Náñez, J. E. (1995). Young infants’ perception of object unity in two-dimensional displays. Infant Behavior & Development, 18, 133143.
  • Johnson, S. P., & Shuwairi, S. M. (2009). Learning and memory facilitate predictive tracking in 4-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 122130.
  • Johnson, S. P., Slemmer, J. A., & Amso, D. (2004). Where infants look determines how they see: Eye movements and object perception performance in 3-month-olds. Infancy, 6, 185201.
  • Kagan, J. (2008). In defense of qualitative changes in development. Child Development, 79, 16061624.
  • Kellman, P. J., & Arterberry, M. E. (1998). The cradle of knowledge: Development of perception in infancy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Kellman, P. J., & Spelke, E. S. (1983). Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 483524.
  • Kessen, W., Salapatek, P., & Haith, M. (1972). The visual response of the human newborn to linear contour. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 13, 920.
  • Kuperstein, M. (1988). Neural model of adaptive hand-eye coordination for single postures. Science, 239, 13081311.
  • Mareschal, D., & Bremner, A. J. (2009). Modeling the origins of object knowledge. In B.Hood & L.Santos (Eds.), The origins of object knowledge (pp. 227262). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Mareschal, D., & Johnson, S. P. (2002). Learning to perceive object unity: A connectionist account. Developmental Science, 5, 151185.
  • Mareschal, D., Plunkett, K., & Harris, P. (1999). A computational and neuropsychological account of object-oriented behaviors in infancy. Developmental Science, 2, 306317.
  • Moore, D. S., & Johnson, S. P. (2008). Mental rotation in human infants: A sex difference. Psychological Science, 19, 10631066.
    Direct Link:
  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.
  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.
  • Quartz, S. R., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1997). The neural basis of cognitive development: An constructivist manifesto. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 537556.
  • Quinn, P. C., & Liben, L. S. (2008). A sex difference in mental rotation in young infants. Psychological Science, 19, 10671070.
    Direct Link:
  • Regolin, L., & Vallortigara, G. (1995). Perception of partly occluded objects by young chicks. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 971976.
  • Rochat, P., & Goubet, N. (1995). Development of sitting and reaching in 5- to 6-month-old infants. Infant Behavior & Development, 18, 5368.
  • Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & the PDP Research Group. (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Schlesinger, M., Amso, D., & Johnson, S. P. (2007). The neural basis for visual selective attention in young infants: A computational account. Adaptive Behavior, 15, 135148.
  • Sekuler, A. B., Lee, J. A. J., & Shettleworth, S. J. (1996). Pigeons do not complete partly occluded figures. Perception, 25, 11091120.
  • Senghas, A., Kita, S., & Özyürek, A. (2004). Children creating core properties of language: Evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua. Science, 305, 17791782.
  • Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701703.
  • Skoczenski, A. M., & Norcia, A. M. (1998). Neural noise limitations on infant visual sensitivity. Nature, 391, 697700.
  • Slater, A. (1995). Visual perception and memory at birth. In C.Rovee-Collier & L. P.Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research, Vol. 9. (pp. 107162). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
  • Slater, A., Johnson, S. P., Brown, E., & Badenoch, M. (1996). Newborn infants’ perception of partly occluded objects. Infant Behavior & Development, 19, 145148.
  • Slater, A., Johnson, S. P., Kellman, P. J., & Spelke, E. S. (1994). The role of three-dimensional depth cues in infants’ perception of partly occluded objects. Early Development and Parenting, 3, 187191.
  • Slater, A., Morison, V., Somers, M., Mattock, A., Brown, E., & Taylor, D. (1990). Newborn and older infants’ perception of partly occluded objects. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 3349.
  • Slater, A., Morison, V., Town, C., & Rose, D. (1985). Movement perception and identity constancy in the new-born baby. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 211220.
  • Slater, A., Quinn, P. C., Kelly, D. J., Lee, K., Longmore, C. A., McDonald, P. R., & Pascalis, O. (in press). The shaping of the face space in early infancy: Becoming a native face processor. Child Development Perspectives.
  • Soska, K. C., Adolph, K. A., & Johnson, S. P. (2010). Systems in development: Motor skill acquisition facilitates 3D object completion. Developmental Psychology, 46, 129138.
  • Soska, K. C., & Johnson, S. P. (2008). Development of 3D object completion in infancy. Child Development, 79, 12301236.
  • Spelke, E. S. (1990). Principles of object perception. Cognitive Science, 14, 2956.
  • Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., & Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605632.
  • Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, 8996.
  • Spencer, J. P., Blumberg, M. S., McMurray, B., Robinson, S. R., Samuelson, L. K., & Tomblin, J. B. (2009). Short arms and talking eggs: Why we should no longer abide the nativist-empiricist debate. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 7987.
  • Sperry, R. W. (1963). Chemoaffinity in the orderly growth of nerve fiber patterns and their connections. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 50, 703710.
  • Valenza, E., Leo, I., Gava, L., & Simion, F. (2006). Perceptual completion in newborn human infants. Child Development, 77, 18101821.
  • Valenza, E., Simion, F., Macchi Cassia, V., & Umiltà, C. (1996). Face preference at birth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 892903.
  • Wong, R. O. L. (1999). Retinal waves and visual system development. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 2947.
  • Zeki, S. (1993). A vision of the brain. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.