SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Alvarado, M. C., & Bachevalier, J. (2000). Revisiting the maturation of medial temporal lobe memory functions in primates. Learning & Memory, 7, 244256.
  • Ashby, F. G., Alfonso-Reese, L. A., Turken, A. U., & Waldron, E. M. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. Psychological Review, 105, 442481.
  • Ashby, F. G., & Maddox, T. W. (2005). Human category learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 149178.
  • Bar-Gad, I., Morris, G., & Bergman, H. (2003). Information processing, dimensionality reduction and reinforcement learning in the basal ganglia. Progress in Neurobiology, 71, 439473.
  • Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective test for measuring flexibility and thinking. Journal of General Psychology, 39, 1522.
  • Blair, M. R., Watson, M. R., & Meier, K. M. (2009). Errors, efficiency, and the interplay between attention and category learning. Cognition, 112, 330336.
  • Brown, R. G., & Marsden, C. D. (1988). Internal versus external cues and the control of attention in Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 111, 323345.
  • Buffalo, E. A., Ramus, S. J., Clark, R. E., Teng, E., Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1999). Dissociation between the effects of damage to perirhinal cortex and area TE. Learning & Memory, 6, 572599.
  • Bunge, S. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2006). Brain-based account of the development of rule use in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 118121.
    Direct Link:
  • Carey, S. (1982). Semantic development: State of the art. In E.Wanner & L. R.Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 347389). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L.Hirschfeld & S.Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 169200). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  • Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1996). Science and core knowledge. Philosophy of Science, 63, 515533.
  • Caviness, V. S., Kennedy, D. N., Richelme, C., Rademacher, J., & Filipek, P. A. (1996). The human brain age 7–11 years: A volumetric analysis based on magnetic resonance images. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 726736.
  • Chater, N., & Christiansen, M. H. (2010). Language acquisition meets language evolution. Cognitive Science, 34(7), 11311157.
  • Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
  • Cincotta, C. M., & Seger, C. A. (2007). Dissociation between striatal regions while learning to categorize via observation and via feedback. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 249265.
  • Cook, R. G., & Smith, J. D. (2006). Stages of abstraction and exemplar memorization in pigeon category learning. Psychological Science, 17, 10591067.
    Direct Link:
  • Cools, A. R., Van Den Bercken, J. H. L., Horstink, M. W. I., Van Spaendonck, K. P. M., & Berger, H. J. C. (1984). Cognitive and motor shifting aptitude disorder in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 47, 443453.
  • Dale, P. S., & Fenson, L. (1996). Lexical development norms for young children. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 125127.
  • Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44, 20372078.
  • Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In D. T.Stuss & R. T.Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 466503). London, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Diamond, A., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989). Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget’s AB task: Evidence for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 44, 2440.
  • Van Domburg, P. H. M. E., & Ten Donkelaar, H. J. (1991). The human substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
  • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340347.
  • Fernandez-Ruiz, J., Wang, J., Aigner, T. G., & Mishkin, M. (2001). Visual habit formation in monkeys with neurotoxic lesions of the ventrocaudal neostriatum. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 41964201.
  • Fisher, A. V. (2007). Are developmental theories of learning paying attention to attention? Cognition, Brain, and Behavior, 11, 635646.
  • Fisher, A. V., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2005). When induction meets memory: Evidence for gradual transition from similarity-based to category-based induction. Child Development, 76, 583597.
  • French, R. M., Mareschal, D., Mermillod, M., & Quinn, P. C. (2004). The role of bottom-up processing in perceptual categorization by 3- to 4-month-old infants: Simulations and data. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 382397.
  • Gasser, M., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Learning nouns and adjectives: A connectionist account. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 269306.
  • Gelman, S. A. (1988). The development of induction within natural kind and artifact categories. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 6595.
  • Gelman, R. (1990). Structural constraints on cognitive development: Introduction to a special issue of Cognitive Science. Cognitive Science, 14, 310.
  • Gelman, S. A., & Coley, J. (1991). Language and categorization: The acquisition of natural kind terms. In S. A.Gelman & J. P.Byrnes (Eds.), Perspectives on language and thought: Interrelations in development (pp. 146196). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Gelman, S. A., & Markman, E. (1986). Categories and induction in young children. Cognition, 23, 183209.
  • Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. A.Kuczaj (Ed.), Language development: Vol. 2. Language, thought and culture (pp. 301334). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Giedd, J. N., Snell, J. W., Lange, N., Rajapakse, J. C., Casey, B. J., Kozuch, P. L., Vaituzis, A. C., Vauss, Y. C., Hamburger, S. D., Kaysen, D., & Rapoport, J. L. (1996a). Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: Ages 4–18. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 551560.
  • Giedd, J. N., Vaituzis, A. C., Hamburger, S. D., Lange, N., Rajapakse, J. C., Kaysen, D., Vauss, Y. C., & Rapoport, J. L. (1996b). Quantitative MRI of the temporal lobe, amygdala, and hippocampus in normal human development: Ages 4–18 years. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 366, 223230.
  • Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Development of cortical circuitry and cognitive function. Child Development, 58, 601622.
  • Golinkoff, R. M., Mervis, C. B., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1994). Early object labels: The case for a developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language, 21, 125155.
  • Gureckis, T. M., & Love, B. C. (2004). Common mechanisms in infant and adult category learning. Infancy, 5, 173198.
  • Hammer, R., & Diesendruck, G. (2005). The role of dimensional distinctiveness in children’s and adults’ artifact categorization. Psychological Science, 16, 137144.
    Direct Link:
  • Hammer, R., Diesendruck, G., Weinshall, D., & Hochstein, S. (2009). The development of category learning strategies: What makes the difference? Cognition, 112, 105119.
  • Hoffman, A. B., & Rehder, B. (2010). The costs of supervised classification: The effect of learning task on conceptual flexibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 319340.
  • Imai, M., & Gentner, D. (1997). A cross-linguistic study of early word meaning: Universal ontology and linguistic influence. Cognition, 62, 169200.
  • Jernigan, T. L., Trauner, D. A., Hesselink, J. R., & Tallal, P. A. (1991). Maturation of the human cerebrum observed in vivo during adolescence. Brain, 114, 20372049.
  • Jones, S. S., & Smith, L. B. (2002). How children know the relevant properties for generalizing object names. Developmental Science, 5, 219232.
  • Jones, S. S., Smith, L. B., & Landau, B. (1991). Object properties and knowledge in early lexical learning. Child Development, 62, 499516.
  • Kaminski, J. A., Sloutsky, V. M., & Heckler, A. F. (2008). The advantage of abstract examples in learning math. Science, 230, 454455.
  • Keil, F. C. (1979). Semantic and conceptual development: An ontological perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6, 449476.
  • Kloos, H., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2008). What’s behind different kinds of kinds: Effects of statistical density on learning and representation of categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 5272.
  • Knowlton, B. J., Mangels, J. A., & Squire, L. R. (1996). A neostriatal habit learning system in humans. Science, 273, 13991402.
  • Koutstaal, W., & Schacter, D. L. (1997). Gist-based false recognition of pictures in older and younger adults. Journal of Memory & Language, 37, 555583.
  • Kruschke, J. K. (1992). ALCOVE: An exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. Psychological Review, 99, 2244.
  • Kruschke, J. K. (1993). Human category learning: Implications for back propagation models. Connection Science, 5, 336.
  • Kruschke, J. K. (2001). Toward a unified model of attention in associative learning. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 45, 812863.
  • Lewkowicz, D. J. (2000). Development of intersensory temporal perception: An epigenetic systems/limitations view. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 281308.
  • Lickliter, R., & Bahrick, L. E. (2000). The development of infant intersensory perception: Advantages of a comparative convergent-operations approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 260280.
  • Lombardi, W. J., Andreason, P. J., Sirocco, K. Y., Rio, D. E., Gross, R. E., Umhau, J. C., & Hommer, D. W. (1999). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance following head injury: Dorsolateral fronto-striatal circuit activity predicts perseveration. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 216.
  • Love, B. C., & Gureckis, T. M. (2007). Models in search of a brain. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 90108.
  • Luciana, M., & Nelson, C. A. (1998). The functional emergence of prefrontally-guided working memory systems in four- to eight-year-old children. Neuropsychologia, 36, 273293.
  • Macario, J. F. (1991). Young children’s use of color in classification: Foods and canonically colored objects. Cognitive Development, 6, 1746.
  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276298.
  • Mandler, J. B., Bauer, P. J., & McDonough, L. (1991). Separating the sheep from the goats: Differentiating global categories. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 263298.
  • Mareschal, D., Quinn, P. C., & French, R. M. (2002). Asymmetric interference in 3- to 4-month-olds’ sequential category learning. Cognitive Science, 26, 377389.
  • Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Markman, A. B., & Ross, B. H. (2003). Category use and category learning. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 592613.
  • Markman, A. B., & Stilwell, C. H. (2001). Role-governed categories. Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence, 13, 329358.
  • Marks, W. (1991). Effects of encoding the perceptual features of pictures on memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 566577.
  • Napolitano, A. C., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2004). Is a picture worth a thousand words? The flexible nature of modality dominance in young children. Child Development, 75, 18501870.
  • Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(1–2), 1137.
  • Nomura, E. M., Maddox, W. T., Filoteo, J. V., Ing, A. D., Gitelman, D. R., Parrish, T. B., Mesulam, M. M., & Reber, P. J. (2007). Neural correlates of rule-based and information-integration visual category learning. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 3743.
  • Nomura, E. M., & Reber, P. J. (2008). A review of medial temporal lobe and caudate contributions to visual category learning. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 279291.
  • Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity and the identification categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 3957.
  • Opfer, J. E., & Bulloch, M. J. (2007). Causal relations drive young children’s induction, naming, and categorization. Cognition, 105, 206217.
  • Opfer, J. E., & Siegler, R. S. (2004). Revisiting preschoolers’ living things concept: A microgenetic analysis of conceptual change in basic biology. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 301332.
  • Pfefferbaum, A., Mathalon, D. H., Sullivan, E. V., Rawles, J. M., Zipursky, R. B., & Lim, K. O. (1994). A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Archives of Neurology, 51, 874887.
  • Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 2542.
  • Quinn, P. C., Eimas, P. D., & Rosenkrantz, S. L. (1993). Evidence for representations of perceptually similar natural categories by 3-month-old and 4-month-old infants. Perception, 22, 463475.
  • Rakison, D. H., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2001). Developmental origin of the animate-inanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 209228.
  • Rao, S. M., Bobholz, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Rosen, A. C., Woodley, S. J., Cunningham, J. M., Cox, R. W., Stein, E. A., & Binder, J. R. (1997). Functional MRI evidence for subcortical participation in conceptual reasoning skills. NeuroReport, 8, 19871993.
  • Robinson, C. W., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2004). Auditory dominance and its change in the course of development. Child Development, 75, 13871401.
  • Robinson, C. W., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2007a). Linguistic labels and categorization in infancy: Do labels facilitate or hinder? Infancy, 11, 233253.
  • Robinson, C. W., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2007b). Visual processing speed: Effects of auditory input on visual processing. Developmental Science, 10, 734740.
  • Robinson, C. W., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2008). Effects of auditory input in individuation tasks. Developmental Science, 11, 869881.
  • Robinson, C. W., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2010). Development of cross-modal processing. WIRES: Cognitive Science, 1, 17.
  • Rodman, H. R. (1994). Development of inferior temporal cortex in the monkey. Cerebral Cortex, 4, 484498.
  • Rodman, H. R., Skelly, J. P., & Gross, C. G. (1991). Stimulus selectivity and state dependence of activity in inferior temporal cortex of infant monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 88, 75727575.
  • Rogers, R. D., Andrews, T. C., Grasby, P. M., Brooks, D. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2000). Contrasting cortical and subcortical activations produced by attentional-set shifting and reversal learning in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 142162.
  • Rogers, T. T., & McClelland, J. L. (2004). Semantic cognition: A parallel distributed processing approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Rosch, E. H., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382439.
  • Rueda, M., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J., Gruber, D., Lercari, L., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 10291040.
  • Saffran, J. R., Johnson, E. K., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1999). Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition, 70, 2752.
  • Samuelson, L. K., & Smith, L. B. (1999). Early noun vocabularies: Do ontology, category structure and syntax correspond? Cognition, 73, 133.
  • Seger, C. A. (2008). How do the basal ganglia contribute to categorization? Their roles in generalization, response selection, and learning via feedback. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 265278.
  • Seger, C. A., & Cincotta, C. M. (2002). Striatal activation in concept learning. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 149161.
  • Shannon, C. E., & Weaver, W. (1948). The mathematical theory of communication. Chicago: University of Illinois Press.
  • Shepp, B. E., & Swartz, K. B. (1976). Selective attention and the processing of integral and nonintegral dimensions: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 22, 7385.
  • Sloutsky, V. M. (2003). The role of similarity in the development of categorization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 246251.
  • Sloutsky, V. M., & Fisher, A. V. (2004a). Induction and categorization in young children: A similarity-based model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 166188.
  • Sloutsky, V. M., & Fisher, A. V. (2004b). When development and learning decrease memory: Evidence against category-based induction in children. Psychological Science, 15, 553558.
    Direct Link:
  • Sloutsky, V. M., & Fisher, A. V. (2008). Attentional learning and flexible induction: How mundane mechanisms give rise to smart behaviors. Child Development, 79, 639651.
  • Sloutsky, V. M., Kloos, H., & Fisher, A. V. (2007). When looks are everything: Appearance similarity versus kind information in early induction. Psychological Science, 18, 179185.
    Direct Link:
  • Sloutsky, V. M., & Lo, Y.-F. (1999). How much does a shared name make things similar? Part 1: Linguistic labels and the development of similarity judgment. Developmental Psychology, 35, 14781492.
  • Sloutsky, V. M., Lo, Y.-F., & Fisher, A. (2001). How much does a shared name make things similar? Linguistic labels, similarity and the development of inductive inference. Child Development, 72, 16951709.
  • Sloutsky, V. M., & Napolitano, A. C. (2003). Is a picture worth a thousand words? Preference for auditory modality in young children. Child Development, 74, 822833.
  • Sloutsky, V. M., & Robinson, C. W. (2008). The role of words and sounds in visual processing: From overshadowing to attentional tuning. Cognitive Science, 32, 354377.
  • Sloutsky, V. M., & Spino, M. A. (2004). Naive theory and transfer of learning: When less is more and more is less. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 528535.
  • Smith, L. B. (1989). A model of perceptual classification in children and adults. Psychological Review, 96, 125144.
  • Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S., & Landau, B. (1996). Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism? Cognition, 60, 143171.
  • Smith, J. D., & Kemier-Nelson, D. G. (1984). Overall similarity in adults’ classification: The child in all of us. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 137159.
  • Smith, J. D., Redford, J. S., & Haas, S. M. (2008). Prototype abstraction by monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 390401.
  • Soja, N., Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1991). Ontological categories guide young children’s inductions of word meanings: Object terms and substance terms. Cognition, 38, 179211.
  • Sowell, E. R., & Jernigan, T. L. (1999). Further MRI evidence of late brain maturation: Limbic volume increases and changing asymmetries during childhood and adolescence. Developmental Neuropsychology, 14, 599617.
  • Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Holmes, C. J., Batth, R., Jernigan, T. L., & Toga, A. W. (1999). Localizing age-related changes in brain structure between childhood and adolescence using statistical parametric mapping. NeuroImage, 9, 587597.
  • Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Holmes, C. J., Jernigan, T. L., & Toga, A. W. (1999). In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 859861.
  • Spelke, E. S. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55, 12331243.
  • Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, 8996.
  • Striedter, G. F. (2005). Principles of brain evolution. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
  • Teng, E., Stefanacci, L., Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (2000). Contrasting effects on discrimination learning after hippocampal lesions and conjoint hippocampal-caudate lesions in monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 38533863.
  • Thapar, A., & McDermott, K. B. (2001). False recall and false recognition induced by presentation of associated words: Effects of retention interval and level of processing. Memory and Cognition, 29, 424432.
  • Tipper, S. P., & Driver, J. (2000). Negative priming between pictures and words in a selective attention task: Evidence for semantic processing of ignored stimuli. In M. S.Gazzaniga (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience: A reader (pp. 176187). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
  • Twyan, A. D., & Newcombe, N. S. (2010). Five reasons to doubt the existence of a geometric module. Cognitive Science, 34(7), 13151356.
  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1964). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published in 1934)
  • Wallace, M. T. (2004). The development of multisensory processes. Cognitive Processing, 5, 6983.
  • Wasserman, E. A., Young, M. E., & Cook, R. G. (2004). Variability discrimination in humans and animals: Implications for adaptive action. American Psychologist, 59, 879890.
  • Waxman, S. R., & Markow, D. B. (1995). Words as invitations to form categories: Evidence from 12- 13-month-old infants. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 257302.
  • Welder, A. N., & Graham, S. A. (2001). The influence of shape similarity and shared labels on infants’ inductive inferences about nonobvious object properties. Child Development, 72, 16531673.
  • Whitman, J. R., & Garner, W. R. (1962). Free recall learning of visual figures as a function of form of internal structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 558564.
  • Wilson, C. (1995). The contribution of cortical neurons to the firing pattern of striatal spiny neurons. Cambridge, MA: Bradford.
  • Xu, F. (2002). The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts in infancy. Cognition, 85, 223250.
  • Yamauchi, T., Kohn, N., & Yu, N.-Y. (2007). Tracking mouse movement in feature inference: Category labels are different from feature labels. Memory & Cognition, 35, 852863.
  • Yamauchi, T., Love, B. C., & Markman, A. B. (2002). Learning nonlinearly separable categories by inference and classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 585593.
  • Yamauchi, T., & Markman, A. B. (1998). Category learning by inference and classification. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 124148.
  • Yamauchi, T., & Markman, A. B. (2000). Inference using categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 776795.
  • Yamauchi, T., & Yu, N.-Y. (2008). Category labels versus feature labels: Category labels polarize inferential predictions. Memory & Cognition, 36, 544553.
  • Yoshida, H., & Smith, L. B. (2003). Shifting ontological boundaries: How Japanese- and English speaking children generalize names for animals and artifacts. Developmental Science, 6, 134.
  • Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development, 11, 3763.
  • Zelazo, P. D., Muller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research on Child Development, 68, vii137.
  • Zentall, T. R., Wasserman, E. A., Lazareva, O. F., Thompson, R. K. R., & Rattermann, M. J. (2008). Concept learning in animals. Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 3, 1345.