SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Aglioti, S., Goodale, M. A., & DeSouza, J. F. X. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679685.
  • Allen, H. A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2009). Direct tactile stimulation of dorsal occipito-temporal cortex in a visual agnosic. Current Biology, 19(12), 10441049. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.04.057
  • Ball, K., Smith, D., Ellison, A., & Schenk, T. (2009). Both egocentric and allocentric cues support spatial priming in visual search. Neuropsychologia, 47, 15851591.
  • Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 227247.
  • Block, N. (2007). Consciousness, accessibility and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 481548.
  • Brogaard, B. (2010). Strong representationalism and centered content. Philosophical Studies, 151(3), 373392. DOI 10.1007/s11098-009-9437-z.
  • Bruno, N., & Franz, V. H. (2009). When is grasping affected by the Müller-Lyer illusion? A quantitative review. Neuropsychologia, 47, 14211433.
  • Bullier, J., Hupé, J. M., James, A. C., & Girard, P. (2001). The role of feedback connections in shaping the responses of visual cortical neurons. Progress in Brain Research, 134, 193204.
  • Carrasco, M. (2009). Visual attention alters appearance: Psychophysical studies of subjective experience. In P.Wilken, T.Bayne, & A.Cleeremans (Eds.), Oxford companion to consciousness. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Carrasco, M., Ling, S., & Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 308313.
  • Cashdollar, N., Malecki, U., Rugg-Gunn, F. J., Duncan, J. S., Lavie, N., & Duzel, E. (2009). Hippocampus dependent and independent theta-networks of active maintenance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 2049320498.
  • Castiello, U., & Jeannerod, M. (1991). Measuring time to awareness. Neuroreport, 2, 797800.
  • Castiello, U., Paulignan, Y., & Jeannerod, M. (1991). Temporal dissociation of motor responses and subjective awareness. A study in normal subjects. Brain, 114, 26392655.
  • Cavaco, S., Anderson, S. W., Allen, J. S., Castro-Caldas, A., & Damasio, H. (2004). The scope of preserved procedural memory in amnesia. Brain, 127, 18531867.
  • Chalmers, D. J. (1990). Consciousness and cognition. Available at: http://consc.net/papers/c-and-c.html. [accessed on September 15, 2009].
  • Chalmers, D. J. (1996). The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Claeys, K. G., Dupont, P., Cornette, L., Sunaert, S., Van Hecke, P., De Schutter, E., & Orban, G. A. (2004). Color discrimination involves ventral and dorsal stream visual areas. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 803822.
  • Clark, A. (2001). Visual experience and motor action: Are the bonds too tight? The Philosophical Review, 110, 495519.
  • Clark, A. (2007). What reaching teaches: Consciousness, control, and the inner zombie. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 58, 563594.
  • Clark, A. (2009). Perception, action, and experience: Unraveling the golden braid. Neuropsychologia, 47, 14601468.
  • Conway, B. R. (2001). Spatial structure of cone inputs to color cells in alert macaque primary visual cortex (V-1). Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 27682783.
  • Conway, B. R., & Livingstone, M. S. (2006). Spatial and temporal properties of cone signals in alert macaque primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 1082610846.
  • Cooper, L. A., & Schacter, D. L. (1992). Dissociations between structural and episodic representations of visual objects. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 141146.
    Direct Link:
  • Danckert, J., & Goodale, M. A. (2000). Blindsight: A conscious route to unconscious vision. Current Biology, 10(1), 3143.
  • Dassonville, P., & Bala, J. K. (2004). Action, perception and the Roelofs effect: A mere illusion of dissociation. PLoS Biology, 2, 19361945.
  • Decety, J., Grèzes, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E., Grassi, F., & Fazio, F. (1997). Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject’s strategy. Brain, 120, 17631777.
  • Desmurget, M., Rossetti, Y., Prablanc, C., Stelmach, G., & Jeannerod, M. (1995). Integration of sensory cues about hand position used for a goal directed movement. Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 73, 262272.
  • Driver, J., Mattingley, J. B., Rorden, C., & Davis, G. (1997). Extinction as a paradigm measure of attentional bias and restricted capacity following brain injury. In P.Thier & H.-O.Karnath (Eds.), Parietal lobe contributions to orientation in 3D space (pp. 401430). Heidelberg, Germany: Springer-Verlag.
  • Elliott, D., Carson, R. G., Goodman, D., & Chua, R. (1991). Discrete vs. continuous control of manual aiming. Human Movement Science, 10, 393418.
  • Franz, V. H., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2008). Grasping visual illusions: Consistent data and no dissociation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 920950.
  • Franz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11, 2025.
    Direct Link:
  • Gallese, V. (2007). The ‘conscious’ dorsal stream: Embodied simulation and its role in space and action conscious awareness. Psyche, 13, Available at: http://www.theassc.org/files/assc/2666.pdf. [accessed on September 19, 2009].
  • Gazzaniga, M. S., Fendrich, R., & Wessinger, C. M. (1994). Blindsight reconsidered. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 9396.
    Direct Link:
  • Gentilucci, M., Daprati, E., Toni, I., Chieffi, S., & Saetti, M. C. (1995). Unconscious updating of grasp motor program. Experimental Brain Research, 105, 291303.
  • Goodale, M. A., Jakobson, L. S., & Keillor, J. M. (1994). Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements. Neuropsychologia, 32, 11591178.
  • Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15, 2025.
  • Goodale, M. A., Milner, A. D., Jakobson, L. S., & Carey, D. P. (1991). A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them. Nature, 349, 154156.
  • Goodale, M. A., Pelisson, D., & Prablanc, C. (1986). Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature, 320, 748750.
  • Gordon, R. D., & Irwin, D. E. (1996). What’s in an object file – evidence from priming studies. Perception and Psychophysics, 58, 12601277.
  • Grezes, J., Costes, N., & Decety, J. (1998). Top-down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: A PET investigation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 553582.
  • Head, H., & Holmes, H. G. (1911). Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain, 34, 102254.
  • Helbig, H. B., Graf, M., & Kiefer, M. (2006). The role of action representations in visual object recognition. Experimental Brain Research, 174, 221228.
  • Jacob, P., & Jeannerod, M. (2003). Ways of seeing: The scopes and limits of visual cognition. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Jakobson, L. S., & Goodale, M. A. (1989). Trajectories of reaches to prismatically-displaced targets: Evidence for ‘automatic’ visuomotor recalibration. Experimental Brain Research, 78, 575587.
  • Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor cognition: What actions tell the self. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Jeannerod, M., & Jacob, P. (2005). Visual cognition. A new look at the two visual systems model. Neuropsychologia, 43, 301312.
  • Kentridge, R. W., Heywood, C. A., & Weiskrantz, L. (2007). Color contrast processing in human striate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1512915131.
  • Kolb, F. C., & Braun, J. (1995). Blinsight in normal observers. Nature, 377, 336338.
  • Kroustallis, B. (2005). Blindsight. Philosophical Psychology, 18, 3143.
  • Lau, H. C., & Passingham, R. E. (2007). Relative blindsight in normal observers and the neural correlate of visual consciousness. Proceedings of the Natural Academy of Sciences, 103, 1876318768.
  • Leh, S. E., Ptito, A., Schönwiesner, M., Chakravarty, M. M., & Mullen, K. T. (2009). Blindsight mediated by an S-Cone-independent collicular pathway: An fMRI study in hemispherectomized subjects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(4), 670682. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21217
  • Lennie, P. (2000). Color vision: Putting it together. Current Biology, 10, R589R591.
  • Li, W., & Gilbert, C. D. (2002). Global contour saliency and local colinear interactions. Journal of Neurophysiology, 88, 28462856.
  • Li, W., Piëch, V., & Gilbert, C. D. (2006). Contour saliency in primary visual cortex. Neuron, 50, 951962.
  • Locklin, J., & Danckert, J. (2010). Do we have independent visual streams for perception and action? A response. Preprint, http://cogprints.org/6854/
  • Lotto, R. B., & Purves, D. (2000). An empirical explanation of color contrast. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 1283412839.
  • Lyon, D. C., Nassi, J. J., & Callaway, E. M. (2010). A disynaptic relay from superior colliculus to dorsal stream visual cortex in macaque monkey. Neuron, 65, 270279.
  • MacKenzie, C. L., & Iberall, T. (1994). The grasping hand. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
  • Marcel, A. J. (1998). Blindsight and shape perception: deficit of visual consciousness or of visual function? Brain, 121, 15651588.
  • McIntosh, R. D., Dijkerman, H. C., Mon-Williams, M., & Milner, A. D. (2004). Grasping what is graspable: Evidence from visual form agnosia. Cortex, 40, 695702.
  • McIntosh, R. D., & Lashley, G (2008). Matching boxes: Familiar size influences action programming. Neuropsychologia, 46(9), 24412444.
  • McIntosh, R. D., & Schenk, T. (2009). Two visual streams for perception and action: Current trends. Neuropsychologia, 47, 13911396.
  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 8197.
  • Milner, A. D., Dijkerman, H. C., Pisella, L., McIntosh, R. D., Tilikete, C., Vighetto, A., & Rossetti, Y. (2001). Grasping the past: Delay can improve visuomotor performance. Current Biology, 11, 18961901.
  • Milner, D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia, 46, 774785.
  • Morgan, M. J., Mason, A. J., & Solomon, J. A. (1997). Blindsight in normal subjects? Nature, 385, 401402.
  • Natsoulas, T. (1997). Blindsight and consciousness. American Journal of Psychology, 110, 133.
  • Noë, A. (2004). Action in perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Overgaard, M., Fehl, K., Mouridsen, K., Bergholt, B., & Cleeremans, A. (2008). Seeing without seeing? Degraded conscious vision in a blindsight patient. PLoS ONE, 3. Available at: http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0003028. [accessed on February 23, 2010].
  • Padoa-Schioppa, C., & Assad, J. A. (2008). The representation of economic value in the orbitofrontal cortex is invariant for changes of menu. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 95102.
  • Paulignan, Y., MacKenzie, C. L., Marteniuk, R. G., & Jeannerod, M. (1991). Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements. 1. The effect of changing object position. Experimental Brain Research, 83, 502512.
  • Peacocke, C. (1983). Sense and content. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Pelisson, D., Prablanc, C., Goodale, M. A., & Jeannerod, M. (1986). Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb. II. Evidence of fast unconscious processes correcting the trajectory of the hand to the final position of a double-step stimulus. Experimental Brain Research, 62, 303311.
  • Polyanskii, V. B., Alymkulov, D. E., Evtikhin, D. V., & Sokolov, E. N. (2008). Assessment of brightness and color differences by neurons in the superior colliculus of the rabbit. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 38, 971983.
  • Prablanc, C., Echallier, J. F., Jeannerod, M., & Komilis, E. (1979). Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target. II Static and dynamic visual cues in the control of hand movement. Biological Cybernetics, 35, 183187.
  • Rogers, G., Smith, D. T., & Schenk, T. (2009). Delayed actions do not bypass the dorsal stream. Neuropsychologia, 47, 15461552.
  • Rossetti, Y. (1998). Implicit short-lived motor representations of space in brain damaged and healthy subjects. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 520558.
  • Rossetti, Y., Stelmach, G., Desmurget, M., Prablanc, C., & Jeannerod, M. (1994). The effect of viewing the static hand prior to movement onset on pointing kinematics and variability. Experimental Brain Research, 101, 323330.
  • Schenk, T. (2006). An allocentric rather than perceptual deficit in patient D.F. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 13451347.
  • Schenk, T., & McIntosh, R. D. (2010). Do we have independent visual streams for perception and action? Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 5161.
  • Schenk, T., & Milner, A. D. (2006). Concurrent visuomotor behaviour improves form discrimination in a patient with visual form agnosia. European Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 14951503.
  • Schilder, P. (1935). The image and appearance of the human body. New York: International Universities Press.
  • Silvanto, J. (2008). A re-evaluation of blindsight and the role of striate cortex (V1) in visual awareness. Neuropsychologia, 46, 28692871.
  • Silvanto, J., Cowey, A., Lavie, N., & Walsh, V. (2005). Striate cortex (V1) activity gates awareness of motion. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 143144.
  • Silvanto, J., Cowey, A., Lavie, N., & Walsh, V. (2007). Making the blindsighted see. Neuropsychologia, 45, 33463350.
  • Silvanto, J., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2008). Inducing conscious perception of colour in blindsight. Current Biology, 18, R950R951.
  • Sparks, D. L. (1999). Conceptual issues related to the role of the superior colliculus in the control of gaze. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9, 698707.
  • Stevens, C. F. (2004). Preserving properties of object shape by computations in primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 1552415529.
  • Stoeric, P., & Cowey, A. (1992). Wavelength discrimination in blindsight. Brain, 115, 42544.
  • Tye, M. (1996). Perceptual experience is a many-layered thing. Philosophical Issues, 7, Perception, 117126.
  • Vishton, P. M., & Fabre, E. (2003). Effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion on different behaviors: One- and two-handed grasping; one- and two-handed manual estimation; metric and comparative judgment. Spatial Vision, 16, 377392.
  • Weiskrantz, L. (1986). Blindsight: A case study and implications. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Weiskrantz, L. (2009). Is blindsight just degraded normal vision? Experimental Brain Research, 192, 413416.
  • Weiskrantz, L., Barbur, J. L., & Sahraie, A. (1995). Factors affecting conscious versus unconscious visual discrimination with V1. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92, 61226126.
  • Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E. K., Sanders, M. D., & Marshall, J. (1974). Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain, 97, 709728.
  • Westwood, D. A., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Perceptual illusion and the real-time control of action. Spatial Vision, 16, 243254.
  • White, B. J., Boehnke, S. E., Marino, R. A., Itti, L., & Munoz, D. P. (2009). Color-related signals in the primate superior colliculus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1215912166.