SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419439.
  • Bakeman, R. (2005). Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 379384.
  • Bartlett, J. C., & Dowling, W. J. (1980). Recognition of transposed melodies: A key-distance effect in developmental perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 501515.
  • Ben-Haim, M. S., Chajut, E., & Eitan, Z. (2010). Common and rare musical keys are absolutely different: Implicit absolute pitch, exposure effects, and pitch processing. Talk presented at the 11th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, Seattle, WA.
  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2009). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 5.1.43. Available at: http://www.praat.org/. Accessed October 30, 2009.
  • Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433436.
  • Chang, H. W., & Trehub, S. E. (1977). Auditory processing of relational information by young infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 24, 324331.
  • Cooper, R. M. (1974). The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language. A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84107.
  • Cornelissen, F. W., Peters, E., & Palmer, J. (2002). The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 34, 613617.
  • Creel, S. C., Aslin, R. N., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2006). Acquiring an artificial lexicon: Effects of segment type and order information. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 119.
  • Creel, S. C., Aslin, R. N., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). Heading the voice of experience: The role of talker variation in lexical access. Cognition, 108, 633664.
  • Creel, S. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2006). Consequences of lexical stress on learning an artificial lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 32, 1532.
  • Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., & Dolson, R. M. (2004). Absolute pitch, speech, and tone language: Some experiments and a proposed framework. Music Perception, 21, 339356.
  • Dowling, W. J., & Fujitani, D. S. (1971). Contour, interval, and pitch recognition in memory for melodies. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49, 524531.
  • Fiser, J., & Aslin, R. N. (2005). Encoding multi-element scenes: Statistical learning of visual feature hierarchies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 521537.
  • Gjerdingen, R., & Perrott, D. (2008). Scanning the dial: The rapid recognition of music genres. Journal of New Music Research, 37, 93100.
  • Hallett, P. E. (1986). Eye movements. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human performance (pp. 10:1–10:112). New York: Wiley.
  • Halpern, A. R. (1989). Memory for the absolute pitch of familiar songs. Memory & Cognition, 17, 572581.
  • Henthorn, T., & Deutsch, D. (2007). Ethnicity versus early environment: Comment on “Early childhood music education and predisposition to absolute pitch: Teasing apart genes and environment.” American Journal of Medical Genetics, 143A, 102103.
  • Hulse, S. H., Cynx, J., & Humpal, J. (1984). Absolute and relative pitch discrimination in serial pitch perception by birds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 3854.
  • Justus, T., & List, A. (2005). Auditory attention to frequency and time: An analogy to visual local—Global stimuli. Cognition, 98, 3151.
  • Keenan, J. P., Halpern, A. R., Thangaraj, V., Chen, C., Edelman, R. R., & Schlaug, G. (2001). Absolute pitch and planum temporale. Neuroimage, 14, 14021408.
  • Krumhansl, C. L. (2010). Thin slices? of music. Music Perception, 27, 337354.
  • Krumhansl, C. L., & Keil, F. C. (1982). Acquisition of the hierarchy of tonal functions in music. Memory & Cognition, 10, 243251.
  • Levitin, D. J. (1994). Absolute memory for musical pitch: Evidence from the production of learned melodies. Perception and Psychophysics, 56, 414423.
  • Lynch, M. P., Eilers, R. E., Oller, D. K., & Urbano, R. C. (1990). Innateness, experience, and music perception. Psychological Science, 1, 272276.
    Direct Link:
  • MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A., & Hulse, S. H. (1996). Concurrent absolute and relative pitch processing by European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110, 139146.
  • Magnuson, J. S., Tanenhaus, M. K., Aslin, R. N., & Dahan, D. (2003). The time course of spoken word learning and recognition: Studies with artificial lexicons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 202227.
  • McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 186.
  • McMurray, B., & Aslin, R. N. (2004). Anticipatory eye movements reveal infants’ auditory and visual categories. Infancy, 6, 203229.
  • McMurray, B., Clayards, M. A., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2008). Tracking the timecourse of phonetic cue integration during spoken word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15, 10641071.
  • Meyer, L. B. (1967). Music, the arts, and ideas; Patterns and predictions in twentieth-century culture. Chicago: U Chicago Press.
  • Mirman, D., & Magnuson, J. S. (2009). Dynamics of activation of semantically similar concepts during spoken word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 37(7), 10261039.
  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353383.
  • Olejnik, S., & Algina, J. (2003). Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: Measures of effect size for some common research designs. Psychological Methods, 8, 434447.
  • Palmer, C., Jungers, M. K., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Episodic memory for musical prosody. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 526545.
  • Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, language, and the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437442.
  • Petran, L. A. (1932). An experimental study of pitch recognition. Psychological Monographs, 42(6), 1120.
  • Pollack, I. (1952). The information of elementary auditory displays. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 24(6), 745749.
  • Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2003). A further look at the “Language-as-Fixed-Effect Fallacy.” Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 141151.
  • Raaijmakers, J. G. W., Schrijnemakers, J. M. C., & Gremmen, F. (1999). How to deal with “The Language-as-Fixed-Effect Fallacy”: Common misconceptions and alternative solutions. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 416426.
  • Saffran, J. R. (2003). Absolute pitch in infancy and adulthood: The role of tonal structure. Developmental Science, 6, 3749.
  • Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 19261928.
  • Saffran, J. R., & Griepentrog, G. J. (2001). Absolute pitch in infant auditory learning: Evidence for developmental reorganization. Developmental Psychology, 37, 7485.
  • Saffran, J. R., Reeck, K., Niebuhr, A., & Wilson, D. (2005). Changing the tune: The structure of the input affects infants’ use of absolute and relative pitch. Developmental Science, 8, 17.
  • Schellenberg, E. G., Iverson, P., & McKinnon, M. C. (1999). Name that tune: Identifying popular recordings from brief excerpts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 641646.
  • Schellenberg, E. G., & Trehub, S. E. (2003). Good pitch memory is widespread. Psychological Science, 14, 262266.
    Direct Link:
  • Sergeant, D. (1969). Experimental investigation of absolute pitch. Journal of Research in Music Education, 17, 135143.
  • Sergeant, D., & Roche, S. (1973). Perceptual shifts in the auditory information processing of young children. Psychology of Music, 1, 3948.
  • Smith, N. A., & Schmuckler, M. A. (2008). Dial A440 for absolute pitch: Absolute pitch memory by non-absolute pitch possessors. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123, EL77EL84.
  • Spivey, M. J., Grosjean, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). Continuous attraction toward phonological competitors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(29), 1039310398.
  • Swingley, D., Pinto, J. P., & Fernald, A. (1999). Continuous processing in word recognition at 24 months. Cognition, 71, 73108.
  • Takeuchi, A. H., & Hulse, S. H. (1993). Absolute pitch. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 345361.
  • Tanenhaus, M., Spivey-Knowlton, M., Eberhard, K., & Sedivy, J. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 16321634.
  • Taylor, P. (1997). Barfly [Computer program]. Version 1.3b1. Available at: http://www.barfly.dial.pipex.com/. Accessed September 25, 2007.
  • Toiviainen, P., & Krumhansl, C. L. (2003). Measuring and modeling real-time responses to music: The dynamics of tonality induction. Perception, 32, 741766.
  • Trehub, S. E., Schellenberg, E. G., & Kamenetsky, S. B. (1999). Infants’ and adults’ perception of scale structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 965975.
  • Trehub, S. E., Schellenberg, E. G., & Nakata, T. (2008). Cross-cultural perspectives on pitch memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100, 4052.
  • Truitt, F. E., Clifton, C., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1997). The perceptual span and the eye-hand span in sight reading music. Visual Cognition, 4, 143161.
  • Ward, W. D. (1999). Absolute pitch. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), Psychology of music (2nd ed., pp. 265298). New York: Academic Press.
  • Weber, A., & Cutler, A. (2004). Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 125.
  • Weisman, R. G., Njegovan, M., Williams, M. T., Cohen, J., & Sturdy, C. B. (2004). A behavior analysis of absolute pitch: Sex, experience, and species. Behavioural Processes, 66, 289307.
  • Weisman, R. G., Williams, M. T., Cohen, J. S., Njegovan, M. J., & Sturdy, C. B. (2006). The comparative psychology of absolute pitch. In E. A. Wasserman & T. R. Zentall (Eds.), Comparative cognition: Experimental explorations of animal intelligence (pp. 7186). New York: Oxford.