SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Afonso, A., Blum, A., Katz, B. F. G., & Tarroux, P. (2010). Structural properties of spatial representations in blind people: Scanning images constructed from haptic exploration or from locomotion in a 3-D audio virtual environment. Memory & Cognition, 38, 591604.
  • Aguiar, A., & Baillargeon, R. (1999). 2.5-month-old infants’ reasoning about when objects should and should not be occluded. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 116157.
  • Arterberry, M. E., & Bornstein, M. H. (2001). Three-month-old infants’ categorization of animals and vehicles based on static and dynamic attributes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 333346.
  • Bahrick, L. E., Gogate, L. J., & Ruiz, I. (2002). Attention and memory for faces and actions in infancy: The salience of actions over faces in dynamic events. Child Development, 73, 16291643.
  • Baillargeon, R. (1986). Representing the existence and the location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6- and 8-month-old infants. Cognition, 23, 2141.
  • Baillargeon, R. (2004). Infants’ reasoning about hidden objects: Evidence for event-general and event-specific expectations. Developmental Science, 7, 391424.
  • Baillargeon, R., Kotovsky, L., & Needham, A. (1995). The acquisition of physical knowledge in infancy. In D. Sperber, D. Premack & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition (pp. 79116). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E., & Wasserman, S. (1985). Object permanence in 5-month-old infants. Cognition, 20, 191208.
  • Ball, W. (1973).The perception of causality in the infant. Presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia, PA.
  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Barr, R., Rovee-Collier, C., & Learmonth, A. (2011). Potentiation in young infants: The origin of the prior knowledge effect? Memory & Cognition, 39, 625636.
  • Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 107154.
  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577660.
  • Batki, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Connellan, J., & Ahluwalia, J. (2000). Is there an innate gaze module? Evidence from human neonates. Infant Behavior & Development, 23, 223229.
  • Behl-Chadha, G. (1996). Superordinate-like categorical representations in early infancy. Cognition, 60, 104141.
  • Biro, S., & Leslie, A. M. (2007). Infants perception of goal-directed actions: Development through cue-based bootstrapping. Developmental Science, 10, 379398.
  • Bogartz, R. S., Shinskey, J. L., & Speaker, C. (1997). Interpreting infant looking: The event set x event set design. Developmental Psychology, 33, 408422.
  • Bornstein, M. H., & Arterberry, M. E. (2010). The development of object categorization in young children: Hierarchical inclusiveness, age, perceptual attribute, and group versus individual analyses. Developmental Psychology, 46, 350365.
  • Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75, 128.
  • Brandone, A. C., & Wellman, H. M. (2009). You can’t always get what you want: Infants understand failed goal-directed actions. Psychological Science, 20, 8591.
    Direct Link:
  • Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). The importance of eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behaviour. Developmental Psychology, 38, 958966.
  • Brown, A. L. (1990). Domain-specific principles affect learning and transfer in children. Cognitive Science, 14, 107133.
  • Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition, 112, 337342.
  • Call, J. (2001). Chimpanzee social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 388393.
  • Call, J., & Santos, L. R. (in press). Understanding other minds. In J. Mitani, J. Call, P. Kappeler, R. Palombit, & J. Silk (Eds.), The evolution of primate societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 187192.
  • Carey, S. (2000). The origin of concepts. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 3741.
  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. Hirshfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain-specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 169208). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4).
  • Carver, L. J., & Bauer, P. J. (1999). When the event is more than the sum of its parts: Nine-month-olds’ long-term ordered recall. Memory, 7, 147174.
  • Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579593.
  • Casasola, M., & Cohen, L. B. (2002). Infant spatial categorization of containment, support, or tight-fit spatial relations. Developmental Science, 5, 247264.
  • Chen, Z., Sanchez, R. P., & Campbell, T. (1997). From beyond to within their grasp: The rudiments of analogical problem-solving in 10- and 13-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 790801.
  • Cheries, E. W., Mitroff, S. R., Wynn, K., & Scholl, B. J. (2009). Do the same principles constrain persisting object representations in infant cognition and adult perception?: The bases of continuity and cohesion. In B. Hood & L. Santos (Eds.), The origins of object knowledge (pp. 107134). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Choi, S., McDonough, L., Bowerman, M., & Mandler, J. M. (1999). Early sensitivity to language-specific spatial categories in English and Korean. Cognitive Development, 14, 241268.
  • Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Coldren, J. T., & Colombo, J. (1994). The nature and processes of preverbal learning: Implications from nine-month-old infants’ discrimination problem solving. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, vii75.
  • Collie, R., & Hayne, H. (1999). Deferred imitation by 6- and 9-month-old infants: More evidence for declarative memory. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, 8390.
  • Csibra, G. (2008). Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants. Cognition, 107, 705717.
  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (1998). The teleological origins of mentalistic action explanations: A developmental hypothesis. Developmental Science, 1, 255259.
  • Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Bíró, S., Koós, O., & Brockbank, M. (1999). Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237267.
  • Daum, M. M., Prinz, W., & Aschersleben, G. (2008). Encoding the goal of an object-directed but uncompleted reaching action in 6- and 9-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 11, 607619.
  • Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye-contact in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 96029605.
  • Fraiberg, S. (1968). Parallel and divergent patterns in blind and sighted infants. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 23, 264300.
  • Frye, D., Rawling, P., Moore, C., & Myers, I. (1983). Object-person discrimination and communication at 3 and 10 months. Developmental Psychology, 19, 303309.
  • Garner, W. R. (1962). Uncertainty and structure as psychological concepts. New York: Wiley.
  • Gelman, R. (1990). First principles organize attention to and learning about relevant data: Number and the animate-inanimate distinction as relevant examples. Cognitive Science, 14, 79106.
  • Gelman, R., Durgin, F., & Kaufman, L. (1995). Distinguishing between animates and inanimates: Not by motion alone. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 150184). New York: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.
  • Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7, 155170.
  • Gentner, D., & Namy, L. L. (2006). Analogical processes in language learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 297301.
    Direct Link:
  • Gibbs, R. W. Jr (2005). Embodiment and cognitive science. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Gomez, J. C. (1991). Visual behavior as a window for reading the mind of others in primates. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind (pp. 195207). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
  • Haith, M. M. (1998). Who put the cog in infants cognition? Is rich interpretation too costly? Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 167179.
  • Hamlin, J. K., Newman, G. E., & Wynn, K. (2009). Eight-month-old infants infer unfulfilled goals, despite ambiguous physical evidence. Infancy, 14, 579590.
  • Hespos, S. J., & Baillargeon, R. (2001). Reasoning about containment events in very young infants. Cognition, 78, 207245.
  • Heyes, C. M. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 21, 101134.
  • Hobson, R. P. (2002). The cradle of thought. London: Macmillan.
  • Huttenlocher, J. (1974). The origins of language comprehension. In R. Solso (Ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Johnson, M. H., & Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development: The case of face recognition. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
  • Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition, 109, 224234.
  • Kovacs, A. M., Teglas, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330, 18301834.
  • Leslie, A. M. (1982). The perception of causality in infants. Perception, 11, 173186.
  • Leslie, A. M. (1984). Infant perception of a manual pick-up event. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2, 1932.
  • Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412436.
  • Leslie, A. M. (1994). ToMM, ToBY, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 119148). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Leslie, A. M. (2005). Developmental parallels in understanding minds and bodies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 460462.
  • Liszkowski, U., Schäfer, M., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Prelinguistic infants, but not chimpanzees, communicate about absent entities. Psychological Science, 20, 654659.
    Direct Link:
  • Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2005a). When the ordinary seems unexpected: Evidence for incremental physical knowledge in young infants. Cognition, 95, 297328.
  • Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2005b). Can a self-propelled box have a goal?: Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 1, 601608.
    Direct Link:
  • Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2007). Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions? Cognition, 105, 489512.
  • Luo, Y., Kaufman, L., & Baillargeon, R. (2009). Young infants’ reasoning about physical events involving inert and self-propelled objects. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 441486.
  • Mandler, G. (1967). Organization and memory. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, 1. New York: Academic Press.
  • Mandler, G. (2002). Organization: What levels of processing are levels of. Memory, 10, 333338.
  • Mandler, J. M. (1984). Stories, scripts, and scenes: Aspects of schema theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby II: Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587604.
  • Mandler, J. M. (2004). The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thought. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Mandler, J. M. (2005). How to build a baby III: Image-schemas and the transition to verbal thought. In B. Hampe (Ed.), Image schemas in cognitive linguistics. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
  • Mandler, J. M. (2008). On the birth and growth of concepts. Philosophical Psychology, 21, 207230.
  • Mandler, J. M. (2010). The spatial foundations of the conceptual system. Language and Cognition, 2, 2144.
  • Mandler, J. M., Bauer, P. J., & McDonough, L. (1991). Separating the sheep from the goats: Differentiating global categories. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 263298.
  • Mandler, J. M., Fivush, R., & Reznick, J. S. (1987). The development of contextual categories. Cognitive Development, 2, 339354.
  • Mandler, J. M., & McDonough, L. (1993). Concept formation in infancy. Cognitive Development, 8, 291318.
  • Mandler, J. M., & McDonough, L. (1995). Long-term recall in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 6096.
  • Mandler, J. M., & McDonough, L. (1998a). Studies in inductive inference in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 6096.
  • Mandler, J. M., & McDonough, L. (1998b). On developing a knowledge base in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 34, 12741288.
  • Mandler, J. M., & McDonough, L. (2000). Advancing downward to the basic level. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 379404.
  • McDonough, L. (1999). Early declarative memory for location. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 381402.
  • McDonough, L., Mandler, J. M., McKee, R. D., & Squire, L. (1995). The deferred imitation task as a nonverbal measure of declarative memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92, 75807584.
  • Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). ‘Like me’: A foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science, 10, 126134.
  • Menzel, C. R. (1999). Unprompted recall and reporting of hidden objects by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) after extended delays. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 426434.
  • Michotte, A. E. (19631948). The perception of causality. London: Methuen.
  • Muentener, P., & Carey, S. (2010). Infants’ causal representations of state change events. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 6386.
  • Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: Emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Newcombe, N., Huttenlocher, J., & Learmonth, A. (1999). Infants’ encoding of location in continuous space. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 483510.
  • Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255258.
  • Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (1995). Disorders of semantic memory. In A. D. Baddeley, B. A. Wilson, & F. N. Watts (Eds.), Handbook of memory disorders (pp. 167186). London: Wiley.
  • Pauen, S. (2002). The global-to-basic level shift in infants’ categorical thinking: First evidence from a longitudinal study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26, 492499.
  • Pauen, S., & Träuble, B. (2009). How 7-month-olds interpret ambiguous motion events: Category-based reasoning in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 59, 275295.
  • Penn, D. C., & Povinelli, D. J. (2007). Causal cognition in human and nonhuman animals: A comparative, critical review. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 97118.
  • Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (2005). Infants’ insight into the mind: How deep? Science, 308, 214216.
  • Perone, S., Madole, K. L., Ross-Sheehy, S., Carey, M., & Oakes, L. M. (2008). The relation between infants’ activity with objects and attention. Developmental Psychology, 44, 12421248.
  • Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (2000). Insights into theory of mind from deafness and autism. Mind & Language, 15, 123145.
  • Piaget, J. (1951). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.
  • Povinelli, D. J. (2000). Folk physics for apes: The chimpanzee’s theory of how the world works. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Povinelli, D. J., & Vonk, J. (2003). Chimpanzee minds: Suspiciously human? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 157160.
  • Proffitt, D. R., & Bertenthal, B. I. (1990). Converging operations revisited: Assessing what infants perceive using discrimination measures. Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 111.
  • Quinn, P. C. (2003). Concepts are not just for objects: Categorization of spatial relation information by infants. In D. R. Rakison & L. M. Oakes (Eds.), Early category and concept development (pp. 5076). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Quinn, P. C., Eimas, P. D., & Rosenkrantz, S. L. (1993). Evidence for representations of perceptually similar natural categories by 3-month-old and 4-month-old infants. Perception, 22, 463475.
  • Roberson, D., Davidoff, J., Davies, I. R. L., & Shapiro, L. R. (2005). Color categories: Evidence for the cultural relativity hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 378411.
  • Rochat, P., Morgan, R., & Carpenter, M. (1997). Young infants’ sensitivity to movement information specifying social causality. Cognitive Development, 12, 537561.
  • Rogers, T. T., & McClelland, J. L. (2004). Semantic cognition: A parallel distributed processing approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Samson, D., & Apperly, I. A. (2010). There is more to mind reading than having theory of mind concepts: New directions in theory of mind research. Infant and Child Development, 19, 443454.
  • Sandler, A.-M., & Hobson, R. P. (2001). On engaging with people in early childhood: The case of congenital blindness. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 6, 205222.
  • Santos, L. R. (2004). ‘Core knowledges’: A dissociation between spatiotemporal knowledge and contact-mechanics in a non-human primate? Developmental Science, 7, 167174.
  • Schlottmann, A., & Ray, E. (2010). Goal attribution to schematic animals: Do 6-month-olds perceive biological motion as animate? Developmental Science, 10, 110.
  • Scholl, B. J., & Leslie, A. M. (1999). Explaining the infant’s object concept: Beyond the perception/cognition dichotomy. In E. Lepore & Z. Pylyshyn (Eds.), What is cognitive science? Oxford, England: Blackwell.
  • Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325, 883885.
  • Simion, F., Regolin, L., & Bulf, H. (2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 809813.
  • Sloutsky, V. M. (2010). From perceptual categories to concepts: What develops? Cognitive Science, 34, 12441286.
  • Sodian, B., & Thoermer, C. (2008). Precursors to a theory of mind in infancy: Perspectives for research on autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 2739.
  • Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Metz, U. (2007). Now I see it but you don’t: 14-month-olds can represent another person’s visual perspective. Developmental Science, 10, 199204.
  • Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-ol infants’ perception of others’ actions. Cognition, 96, B1B11.
  • Song, H., Onishi, K. H., Baillargeon, R., & Fisher, C. (2008). Can an agent’s false belief bed corrected by an appropriate communication? Psychological reasoning in 18-month-old infants. Cognition, 109, 295315.
  • Southgate, V., Chevallier, C., & Csibra, G. (2009). Seventeen-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ referential communication. Developmental Science, 12, 10131019.
  • Spelke, E. (1994). Initial knowledge: Six suggestions. Cognition, 50, 431445.
  • Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., & Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605632.
  • Spelke, E. S., & Hespos, S. J. (2002). Conceptual development in infancy: The case of containment. In N. L. Stein, P. J. Bauer, & M. Rabinowitz (Eds.), Representation, memory, and development: Essays in honor of Jean Mandler (pp. 225246). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Spelke, E. S., & Kinsler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, 8996.
  • Srinivasan, N., & Carey, S. (2010). The long and the short of it: On the nature and origin of functional overlap between representations of space and time. Cognition, 116, 217241.
  • Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18, 580586.
    Direct Link:
  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675735.
  • Traugott, E. C. (1978). On the expression of spatio-temporal relations in language. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of human language: Vol. 3. Word structure. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
  • Volkova, A., Trehub, S. E., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2006). Infants’ memory for musical performances. Developmental Science, 9, 583589.
  • Wang, S., Baillargeon, R., & Paterson, S. (2005). Detecting continuity and solidity violations in infancy: A new account and new evidence from covering events. Cognition, 95, 129173.
  • Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy, 11, 271294.
  • Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75, 523541.
  • White, P. A. (1988). Causal processing: Origins and development. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 3652.
  • Whiten, A. (1996). When does smart behaviour reading become mind reading? In P. Carruthers & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theory of mind. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A. (2008). Children acquire emotion categories gradually. Cognitive Development, 23, 291312.
  • Wilcox, T. (1999). Object individuation: Infants’ use of shape, size, pattern, and color. Cognition, 72, 125166.
  • Willatts, P. (1997). Beyond the ‘couch potato’ infant: How infants use their knowledge to regulate action, solve problems, and achieve goals. In G. Bremner, A. Slater, & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Infant development: Recent advances (pp. 109135). Hove, England: Psychology Press.
  • Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 134.
  • Wu, L., & Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Perceptual simulation in conceptual combination: Evidence from property generation. Acta Psychologica, 132, 173189.
  • Xu, F., Carey, S., & Quint, N. (2004). The emergence of kind-based object individuation in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 155190.
  • Zwickel, J., White, S., Coniston, D., Senju, A., & Frith, U. (in press). Exploring the building blocks of social cognition: Spontaneous agency perception and visual perspective taking in autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(5), 564571.