SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Anderson, A., Bader, M., Bard, E., Boyle, E., Doherty, G. M., Garrod, S., Isard, S., Kowtko, J., McAllister, J., Miller, J., Sotillo, C., Thompson, H. S., & Weinert, R. (1991). The HCRC Map Task Corpus. Language and Speech, 34, 351366.
  • Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Aylett, M., & Turk, A. (2004). he smooth signal redundancy hypothesis: A functional explanation for relationships between redundancy, prosodic prominence and duration in spontaneous speech. Language and Speech, 47, 3156.
  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390412.
  • Bailenson, J. N., & Yee, N. (2005). Digital chameleons: Automatic assimilation of nonverbal gestures in immersive virtual environments. Psychological Science, 16, 814819.
    Direct Link:
  • Bard, E. G., & Anderson, A. (1994). The unintelligibility of speech to children: Effects of referent availability. Journal of Child Language, 21, 623648.
  • Bekkering, H., Bruijn, E. R. A. de, Cuijpers, R. H., Newman-Norlund, R., Schie, H. T. van, & Meulenbroek, R. (2009). Joint Action: Neurocognitive Mechanisms Supporting Human Interaction. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 340352.
  • Bernieri, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Interpersonal coordination: Behavior matching and interactional synchrony. In R. S. Feldman & B. Rime (Eds.), Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior: Studies in emotion & social interaction (pp. 401432). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. New York: Pergamon.
  • Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & Cleland, A. A. (2000). Syntactic coordination in dialogue. Cognition, 75, B1325.
  • Cappella, J., & Planalp, S. (1981). Talk and silence sequences in informal conversations III: Interspeaker influence. Human Communications Research, 7, 117132.
  • Carletta, J., Isard, A., Isard, S., Kowtko, J., Doherty-Sneddon, G., & Anderson, A. (1997). The reliability of a dialogue structure coding scheme. Computational Linguistics, 23, 1331.
  • Cator, L. J., Arthur, B. J., Harrington, L. C., & Hoy, R. R. (2009). Harmonic convergence in the love songs of the dengue vector mosquito. Science, 323, 10771079.
  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893910.
  • Chartrand, T. L., Maddux, W, & Lakin, J. (2005). Beyond the perception-behavior link: The ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of nonconscious mimicry. In R. Hassin, J. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscious (pp. 334361). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Condon, W.S., & Ogston, W.D. (1967). A segmentation of behavior. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 5, 221235.
  • Dale, R., Kirkham, N. Z., & Richardson, D. C. (2011). The dynamics of reference and shared visual attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 355.
  • Dale, R., & Spivey, M. J. (2006). Unraveling the dyad: Using recurrence analysis to explore patterns of syntactic coordination between children and caregivers in conversation. Language Learning, 56, 391430.
  • Dale, R., Warlaumont, A. S., & Richardson, D. C. (2011). sequential analysis for behavioral streams. International Journal Bifurcation and Chaos, 21, 11531161.
  • Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perception-behavior expressway: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 33 (pp. 140). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Hager, J. C. (Eds.). (2002). Facial Action Coding System [E-book]. Salt Lake city, UT: Research Nexus.
  • Ferreira, F. (2003). The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 164203.
  • Flecha-García, M. L. (2010). Eyebrow raises in dialogue and their relation to discourse structure, utterance function and pitch accents in English. Speech Communication, 52, 542554.
  • Fowler, C. A., Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Shockley, K. (2008). Language use, coordination, and the emergence of cooperative action. in A. Fuchs & V. Jirsa (Eds.), Coordination: Neural, behavioral and social dynamics (pp. 281307). Berlin: Springer.
  • Gambetta, D. G. (Ed.). (1988). Trust: Making and breaking cooperative relations. New York: Blackwell.
  • Garrod, S., & Anderson, A. (1987). Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition, 27, 181218.
  • Garrod, S., & Doherty, G. (1994). Conversation, co-ordination and convention: An empirical investigation of how groups establish linguistic conventions. Cognition, 53, 181215.
  • Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2009). Joint action, interactive alignment, and dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 292304.
  • Giles, H., Coupland, N., & Coupland, J. (1991). Communication, context, and consequence. In H. Giles, N. Coupland, & J. Coupland (Eds.), Contexts of accommodation. (pp. 4968). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Giles, H., & Powesland, P. F. (1975). Speech styles and social evaluation. New York: Academic Press.
  • Grammer, K., Kruck, K. B., & Magnusson, M. S. (1998). The courtship dance: Patterns of nonverbal synchronization in opposite-sex encounters. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22(1), 329.
  • Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge, England: Cambrige University Press.
  • Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27, 949960.
  • Hurley, S., & Chater, N. (Eds.). (2005). Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Jaeger, T. F. (2006). Redundancy and syntactic reduction in spontaneous speech. Ph.D. thesis, Stanford University, Palo Alto.
  • Jaeger, T. F. (2010). Redundancy and reduction: Speakers manage syntactic information density. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 2362.
  • Kugler, P.N., Kelso, J. A. S., & Turvey, M. T. (1980). On the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures: I. Theoretical lines of convergence. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Reqiun (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 149). New York: North–Holland.
  • Levy, R., & Jaeger, T. F. (2007). Speakers optimize information density through syntactic reduction. In B. Schlökopf, J. Platt, & T. Hoffman (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing systems (NIPS) 19 (pp. 849856). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Lieberman, P. (1963). Some effects of the semantic and grammatical context on the production and perception of speech. Language and Speech, 6, 172175.
  • Lindblom, B. (1990). Explaining variation: A sketch of the H and H theory. In W. Hardcastle & A. Marchal (Eds.), Speech production and speech modelling (pp. 403439). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.
  • Littell, R. C, Stroup, W. W, & Freund, R. J. (2002). SAS for linear models. Cary, NC: SAS Publishing.
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Bangerter, A. (2010). Effects of ambiguous gestures and language on the time course of reference resolution. Cognitive Science, 34, 15171529.
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Crossley, S. A. (2006). Dialog act classification using N-Gram algorithms. In G. Sutcliffe & R. Goebel (Eds.), Proceedings of the International Florida Artificial Intelligence Research Society (pp. 758763). Menlo Park, CA: AAAI Press.
  • Louwerse, M.M., & Mitchell, H.H. (2003). Towards a taxonomy of a set of discourse markers in dialog: A theoretical and computational linguistic account. Discourse Processes, 35, 199239.
  • Marwan, N., Romano, M. C., Thiel, M., & Kurths, J. (2007). Recurrence plots for the analysis of complex systems. Physics Reports, 438, 237329.
  • McDowall, J. J. (1978). Interactional synchrony: A reappraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 963975.
  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 7578.
  • Neumann, R., & Strack, F. (2000). “Mood contagion”: The automatic transfer of mood between persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 211223.
  • Partridge, B. L. (1981). Internal dynamics and the interrelations of fish in schools. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 144, 313325.
  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Towards a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169226.
  • Platek, S. M., Critton, S. R., Myers, T. E., Gallup, G. G., & others. (2003). Contagious yawning: The role of self-awareness and mental state attribution. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 223227.
  • Richardson, D. C.,& Dale, R. (2005). Looking to understand: The coupling between speakers’ and listeners' eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognitive Science, 29, 3954.
  • Richardson, D. C., Dale, R., & Kirkham, N. (2007a). The art of conversation is coordination: Common ground and the coupling of eye movements during dialogue. Psychological Science, 18, 407413.
    Direct Link:
  • Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R., Goodman, J., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007b). Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Human Movement Science, 26, 867891.
  • Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2005). Effects of visual and verbal interaction on unintentional interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 31, 6279.
  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169192.
  • Rogers, Y., & Ellis, J. (1994). Distributed cognition: An alternative framework for analysing and explaining collaborative working. Journal of Information Technology, 9, 119128.
  • Schegloff, E. A., & Sacks, H. (1973). Opening up closings. Semiotica, 8, 289327.
  • Schiffrin, D. (1987). Discourse markers. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 7076.
  • Semin, G. R., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Grounding social cognition: Synchronization, entrainment, and coordination. in G.R. Semin & E.R. Smith (Eds.), Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 119147). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Shockley, K., Richardson, D. C., & Dale, R. (2009). Conversation and coordinative structures. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 305319.
  • Shockley, K., Baker, A., Richardson, M. J., & Fowler, C. A. (2007). Articulatory constraints on interpersonal postural coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 201208.
  • Shockley, K., Santana, M., & Fowler, C. A. (2003). Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 326332.
  • Van Baaren, R., Janssen, L., Chartrand, T., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2009). Where is the love? Social aspects of mimicry Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, Biological sciences, 364, 23812389.
  • Van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Steenaerts, B., & van Knippenberg, A. (2003). Mimicry for money: Behavioral consequences of imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 393398.
  • Wang, Y., Newport, R., & Hamilton, A. (2011). Eye contact enhances mimicry of intransitive hand movements. Biology Letters, 7, 71.
  • Ward, A. J. W., Axford, S., & Krause, J. (2002). Mixed-species shoaling in fish: the sensory mechanisms and costs of shoal choice. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 52, 182187.
  • Webb, J. T. (1969). Subject speech rates as a function of interviewer behavior. Language and Speech, 12, 5467.