SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

Keywords:

  • ARNTL;
  • association;
  • bipolar I disorder;
  • circadian;
  • genetic;
  • linkage;
  • PER3;
  • schizoaffective disorder;
  • schizophrenia;
  • TIMELESS

Abstract

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Materials and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments
  8. Supporting Information

We hypothesize that circadian dysfunction could underlie, at least partially, the liability for bipolar 1 disorder (BD1). Our hypothesis motivated tests for the association between the polymorphisms of genes that mediate circadian function and liability for BD1. The US Caucasian patients with BD1 (DSM-IV criteria) and available parents were recruited from Pittsburgh and surrounding areas (n = 138 cases, 196 parents) and also selected from the NIMH Genetics Collaborative Initiative (n = 96 cases, 192 parents). We assayed 44 informative single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) from eight circadian genes in the BD1 samples. A population-based sample, specifically cord blood samples from local live births, served as community-based controls (n = 180). It was used as a contrast for genotype and haplotype distributions with those of patients. US patients with schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder (SZ/SZA, n = 331) and available parents from Pittsburgh (n = 344) were assayed for a smaller set of SNPs based on the results from the BD1 samples. Modest associations with SNPs at ARNTL (BmaL1) and TIMELESS genes were observed in the BD1 samples. The associations were detected using family-based and case–control analyses, albeit with different SNPs. Associations with TIMELESS and PERIOD3 were also detected in the Pittsburgh SZ/SZA group. Thus far, evidence for association between specific SNPs at the circadian gene loci and BD1 is tentative. Additional studies using larger samples are required to evaluate the associations reported here.

The inherent cyclical nature of bipolar 1 disorder (BD1) invites analogies with circadian variation. Patients with BD1 who have relapsed manifest marked changes in diurnal activities such as sleep, activity, appetite and the diurnal secretion of hormones (Kennedy et al. 1996; Kripke et al. 1978; Leibenluft et al. 1996; Mendlewicz 1982; Nurnberger et al. 1990; Sarai & Kayano 1968; Schreiner et al. 2001). Normalization of these abnormalities usually accompanies clinical stabilization, and mood-stabilizing drugs are known to modulate circadian rhythms (Abe et al. 2000; Basturk et al. 2001; Campbell et al. 1989; Manji et al. 2001; Mellerup et al. 1976). Hence, circadian abnormalities have long been suggested to be critically involved in the pathogenesis of BD1, especially rapid cycling BD1 (Healy 1987; Mitterauer 2000).

Our recent work supports the circadian hypothesis. We compared ‘morningness–eveningness’ (M/E), a stable, heritable trait reflecting circadian phase among patients with BD1, unscreened controls and patients with schizophrenia (SZ) or schizoaffective disorder (SZA). Scores on the composite scale (CS) (Smith et al. 1989), which was used to evaluate M/E, were significantly different among BD1 cases compared with the controls or SZ/SZA patients, when age was considered. Younger BD1 patients and those with rapid mood swings were more significantly likely to have lower CS scores. Thus, distinct patterns of M/E were noted among BD1 patients and among BD1 subgroups (Mansour et al. in press). The impact of medication, mood state and chronicity on CS scores is presently being evaluated.

The biology and genetics of human circadian rhythms have been clarified considerably. Circadian rhythms originate in the suprachiasmatic nucleus. They are regulated by an intrinsic circadian oscillator based on interlinked autoregulatory transcriptional/translational feedback loops (Cermakian & Boivin 2003). Self-regulatory feedback loops are thus set up and entrain circadian rhythms in numerous tissues in mammals. A large number of proteins critical for circadian function have been identified, and intensive efforts are in progress to identify other agents. These proteins mediate the feedback loops underlying circadian rhythms through intricate interactions (Holzberg & Albrecht 2003).

Although the phase and amplitude measured from circadian rhythms undoubtedly reflect a mixture of influences both from the endogenous circadian pacemaker and from exogenous influences such as posture, behavior and environment, there are clear, significant heritable influences on circadian function. For example, twin studies have suggested a heritability of approximately 74% on growth hormone secretion during the wake period and, to a certain extent, on the 24 h secretion (Mendlewicz et al. 1999). Heritable influences on other circadian measures are also available. Twin studies have suggested a heritability of approximately 44% for M/E, a measure based on preference for mornings or evenings (Vink et al. 2001). This convenient measure of circadian function is associated with interindividual differences in the phase (timing) of physiological circadian rhythms, for example, plasma cortisol and core body temperature, as well as behavioral rhythms related to eating, exercise and performance (Baehr et al. 2000; Horne & Ostberg 1976; Matsumoto et al. 1996; Monk et al. 1991; Nebel et al. 1996).

Efforts to understand the phenotypic variation associated with circadian gene polymorphisms are under way. A missense mutation in the PERIOD2 (PER2) gene appears to cause familial advanced sleep-phase syndrome in some pedigrees (Toh et al. 2001). Variations in the CLOCK and PER3 genes are associated with the delayed sleep-phase syndrome and sleep dysregulation in mood disorders (Archer et al. 2003; Ebisawa et al. 2001; Serretti et al. 2003). Polymorphisms at the CLOCK locus could also confer susceptibility to seasonal affective disorder (SAD) (Johansson et al. 2003). A polymorphism in the 3′ flanking polymorphism of the CLOCK gene has been investigated extensively. It was reported to be associated with diurnal preference (Katzenberg et al. 1998), but the association could not be detected in a subsequent study (Robilliard et al. 2002). This polymorphism does not appear to confer susceptibility to major depressive disorder (Desan et al. 2000), although a recent study among BD1 patients suggested an association with the recurrence of manic or depressive episodes (Benedetti et al. 2003). Thus, associations between circadian gene polymorphisms and mood morbidity are plausible.

We addressed the possibility that the genetic susceptibility to BD may be attributed partly to variations in genes that mediate circadian function. If established, such associations would provide additional evidence for circadian disruption in the pathogenesis of BD. To obtain credible evidence, we investigated associations using family-based and case–control samples. It has been suggested that some genetic susceptibility factors may be shared between BD and SZ, and circadian dysfunction has been reported among patients with SZ/SZA (Katz et al. 2001). Hence, we also investigated patients with SZ/SZA.

Materials and methods

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Materials and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments
  8. Supporting Information

Clinical data

Pittsburgh-based samples

When appropriate, written informed consent was obtained from participants, according to the guidelines of the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board. Individuals with BD1, SZ or SZA (DSM-IV criteria) and their available parents participated. Details of ascertainment and diagnosis have been described elsewhere (Chowdari et al. 2002; Ranade et al. 2003). Briefly, consenting patients were interviewed using the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies (DIGS), a structured diagnostic interview schedule (Nurnberger et al. 1994). Additional clinical information was obtained from available clinical records and from relatives. This information was synthesized and a consensus diagnosis was assigned. The parents provided blood samples, but detailed diagnostic interviews were not conducted. To contrast genotype distributions of the probands with those from the population, we obtained cord blood samples from live births at Magee-Women's Hospital, Pittsburgh. No information apart from ethnicity was available for these samples.

NIMH Collaborative Genetics Initiative family-based sample

Pedigrees having probands with BD1 (DSM-IV criteria) were ascertained at Indiana University, Johns Hopkins University, Washington University and the Clinical Neurogenetics Branch of the NIMH Intramural Research Program. The DIGS was the primary interview schedule. The families were ascertained if they included two or more affected first-degree relatives. Pedigrees are sequentially extended through first-degree relatives of specific individuals with specific diagnoses. Pedigrees in which both parents of the proband have SAB (SZA, bipolar type) or BD1 are excluded.

Laboratory

Genomic DNA was extracted from venous blood samples using the phenol–chloroform method.

SNP selection and genotype assays

We selected 46 single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) at eight circadian genes: aryl hydrocarbon receptor nuclear translocator-like BmaL1 (ARNTL), circadian locomotor output cycles kaput protein (CLOCK), Period1, Period2 and Period3 (PER1, PER2 and PER3), Cryptochrome1 and 2 (CRY1 and CRY2) and TIMELESS (TIMELESS) (Tables 1 and 2). Additional details are available online (see Supplementary material).

Table 1.  Candidate genes, SNPs and overall results of association analyses
      SZ/SZA
GeneLocationGenomic size (bp)*Number of SNPs analyzedPittsburgh BD1 CCBD1 TDTCCTDT
  1. BD1, bipolar I disorder [note: only BD1 cases from the Pittsburgh sample were used for association analysis using the Trends test (CC), whereas the transmission disequilibrium test (TDT) included the Pittsburgh and NIMH samples]; +, significant association detected (P < 0.05) (corrections for multiple comparisons not applied); –, significant association not detected; NA: not analyzed; SZ/SZA, schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder.

  2. The following SNPs were genotyped in the SZ/SZA and the NIMH samples: CLOCK: rs6828570; PER1: rs10462024; PER2: rs2304670. No SNPs were genotyped at CRY1 and CRY2.

  3. *Genomic sequence, including exons and introns (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov, build 33).

BmaL1 (ARNTL)11p15109 46010++
Clock (CLOCK)4q12114 33710
Period1 (PER1)17p13.1-17p1215 8886
Period2 (PER2)2q37.344 4062
Period3 (PER3)1p36.2360 8536+++
Cryptochrome1 (CRY1)12q23-q24.1102 1804NANA
Cryptochrome2 (CRY2)11p11.270 1882NANA
TIMELESS (TIMELESS)12q12-q1333 0104++
Table 2.  Analysis of single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) at ARNTL, PERIOD3 and TIMELESS
     Pittsburgh SZ/SZA
GeneSNP IDAllele*Pittsburgh Trends testBD1 TDTTrends testTDT
  • BD1: bipolar I disorder [note: only BD1 cases from the Pittsburgh sample were used for association analysis using the Trends test (CC), whereas the transmission disequilibrium test (TDT) included the Pittsburgh and NIMH samples]; NA, not analyzed; SZ/SZA: schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder.

  • *

    The alleles at each SNP are listed. Elsewhere, the alleles are numbered ‘1’ or ‘2’ in the order listed here. Corrections for multiple comparisons were not applied.

ARNTLrs2279287A/G0.0560.3220.1730.348
 rs1481892C/G0.0180.452NANA
 rs7107287G/T0.0330.047NANA
 rs4757142G/A0.00080.597NANA
 rs895682A/C0.2300.0080.8460.697
 rs1982350T/C0.0050.1620.2550.561
 rs2896635A/T0.1620.2350.7720.933
 rs2278749C/T0.1610.162NANA
 rs969486C/T0.5010.994NANA
 rs2290035T/A0.0900.1810.6890.935
PER3rs228729C/T0.0800.8360.0280.914
 rs10462018C/T0.7800.3030.8780.197
 rs10462020T/G0.0700.7840.0800.317
 rs2859387A/G0.0390.8030.1680.020
 rs10462021C/T0.2810.6800.9130.485
 rs4908699A/G0.4700.3980.1610.352
TIMELESSrs2279665G/C0.7170.047NANA
 rs774026C/T0.8350.3140.3520.039
 rs2291738A/G0.3280.0290.2560.126
 rs2291739A/G0.8120.089NANA
SNP selection

We resequenced all exons, exon–intron boundaries, selected intronic fragments, 5′ upstream sequences (approximately 4 kbp) and 3′ downstream sequences (3 kbp) for all eight circadian genes (Fig. 1). In addition, all introns were resequenced at TIMELESS, PER1 and CRY1: amplicons approximately 500 bp were generated using the polymerase chain reaction (PCR) for genomic DNA samples pooled from 93 cases and a separate pool of 93 community-based controls. The amplified fragments were sequenced using an ABI 3700 DNA sequencer (ABI Inc.; Foster City, CA, USA). We aimed for an average spacing of 1 SNP/25 kbp. SNP selection was also contingent on informativeness (minor allele frequency > 5%) and on assay availability. Among 48 SNPs that were analyzed initially, four were discarded because of inconsistent genotyping assays (rs2304675 A/G at PER2, rs2292912 G/C at CRY2 and rs707465 A/G and rs1773134 A/T at PER3 gene). Thus, 44 SNPs were used. They included 31 SNPs listed in public databases (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/genome/guide/human/) and 13 novel SNPs (details online at http://www.pitt.edu/~nimga, see Supplementary material). Genomic DNA from a panel of six individuals was resequenced for each SNP. These samples served as positive controls for subsequent genotype assays.

image

Figure 1. Structure of ARNTL, PERIOD3 and TIMELESS genes.

Download figure to PowerPoint

Genotype assays

Assays were based on multiplex PCR followed by single-base extension analysis (SnaPshot assays, ABI Inc.; see Supplementary material). We denoted alleles at each polymorphism as ‘1’ or ‘2’ for convenience. All allele calls were rechecked independently, blind to diagnostic status. Genotypes were then evaluated for Mendelian consistency using pedcheck (O'Connell & Weeks 1998). In case of any discrepancy, samples were retyped. All clinical and genetic data were double checked to guard against data entry errors.

Statistical analysis

Transmission distortion at individual SNPs was tested initially using the transmission disequilibrium test (TDT) as implemented in the genehunter software package (Kruglyak 1999). Using the count of transmitted alleles from heterozygous parents to their affected offspring, the TDT assesses distortion from the Mendelian expectation of equal transmission rates for all alleles at a locus. The TDT detects association in the presence of linkage, and its statistic cannot be confounded by population substructure (Ewens & Spielman 1995; Spielman et al. 1993). The possibility of transmission bias could be evaluated in three family-based samples: families containing individuals diagnosed with BD1 from Pittsburgh; families containing individuals diagnosed with BD1 from NIMH and families containing individuals diagnosed with SZ from Pittsburgh. From a purely phenomenological point of view, there would be no reason to analyze the bipolar samples separately; however, there would also be little reason to combine the SZ and bipolar samples. For this reason, we analyzed transmission bias of the bipolar samples jointly, taking into account the origin of the samples, and we analyzed transmission bias for SZ separately. For the joint analysis of each locus, we recorded transmission of the same allele from parents to their affected offspring for each sample. We then fit a logit model in which congruence with Mendelian transmission was tested, as well as whether the transmission rate was the same for samples from both sites.

When significant transmission distortion was detected, we explored the bounds of the associated genomic region by evaluating the transmission of haplotypes composed of two to four contiguous SNPs from the gene (i.e. a ‘sliding window’ to two to four SNPs; Nair et al. 2000). When significant distortion of haplotype transmission was detected, we also analyzed the data using transmit for a global test of association for haplotypes (Clayton 1999); its test statistic, for h haplotypes, is distributed as chi-square with (h − 1) degrees of freedom under the null hypothesis of no transmission distortion. transmit has the advantage of using all data to estimate haplotype frequencies and account for haplotype uncertainty by averaging over all possible configurations of parental haplotypes.

To evaluate whether the genotype distribution differed significantly among individuals diagnosed with BD1 vs. the population in general, we contrasted the observed genotyped distribution for cases with the sample from neonates by using the Cochran–Armitage trend test (Devlin et al. 2001). This test evaluates whether the counts of the minor allele at a locus (0, 1 or 2 minor alleles) can predict status, case or population sample; its test statistic is chi-square distributed with df = 1. Again, when a significant difference was detected, we explored the bounds of the associated genomic region by evaluating differences in haplotype distributions when they were composed of two to four contiguous SNPs from the gene. These sliding window analyses were conducted using snpem (Fallin et al. 2001), which uses the expectation-maximization algorithm to estimate haplotype frequencies for each sample. An empirical omnibus statistic is produced based on the permutation of status with a user-specified number of permutations – in this case 10 000.

The reader will recognize the exploratory nature of the analyses. For this reason, we have not corrected for multiple comparisons.

Results

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Materials and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments
  8. Supporting Information

All genotype distributions were in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium for case-, parental- and population-based samples, with the exception of rs1481892 at ARNTL. A heterozygote deficit was noted for this SNP in the neonatal sample (P = 0.030) and in parents of NIMH patients (P = 0.042). For details of all analyses reported herein, including linkage disequilibrium analyses, please see Results online (http://www.pitt.edu/~nimga).

BD1 sample

The Pittsburgh BD1 sample was composed of 138 Caucasian cases. Both parents were available for 61 probands and one parent was available for each of 74 cases. From the NIMH sample, 96 case–parent trio families were selected. When we analyzed transmission distortion of the bipolar samples jointly, we observed significant transmission bias at four loci: ARNTL rs7107287 (χ2 = 3.92, df = 1, P = 0.0477); ARNTL rs895682 (χ2 = 7.00, df = 1, P = 0.0082); TIMELESS rs2279665 (χ2 = 3.94, df = 1, P = 0.0472) and TIMELESS rs2291738 (χ2 = 4.73, df = 1, P = 0.0296). Members of these pairs of SNPs are in linkage disequilibrium (D′ > 0.9) (see Supplementary data online http://www.pitt.edu/~nimga). Another locus in TIMELESS showed substantial but not quite significant transmission bias rs2291739 (χ2 = 2.89, df = 1, P = 0.0890). Neither these loci nor any other locus tested showed significant differences between sites in the transmission rate.

For the contrasts of genotype distributions for individuals from the Pittsburgh sample diagnosed with BD1 vs. the population in general, significant differences were detected at four loci: ARNTL SNPs rs1481892 (P = 0.018); rs7107287 (P = 0.033); rs4757142 (P = 0.00086) and rs1982350 (P = 0.005) and PER3 SNP rs2859387 (P = 0.039).

To explore the bounds of the associated genomic region for each of three associated genes, ARNTL, TIMELESS and PER3, we evaluated both transmission and distribution of haplotypes composed of two to four contiguous SNPs from each gene. The set of contiguous SNPs was formed by a sliding window of SNPs across the gene.

For ARNTL, these analyses revealed over-transmission of haplotypes encompassing five SNPs rs1982350, rs2896635, rs2278749, rs969486 and rs2290035. However, global tests of haplotypes involving these SNPs revealed no significant association (χ2 = 9.18, df = 11, P = 0.61). For TIMELESS, these analyses revealed over-transmission of haplotypes encompassing four SNPs rs2279665, rs774026, rs2291738 and rs2291739. Global tests of haplotypes involving these SNPs revealed a significant association (χ2 = 9.62, df = 3, P = 0.02). Finally, consistent with the single-locus results, our exploratory analyses revealed no transmission bias for PER3.

For ARNTL, exploratory analyses based on haplotype distributions in individuals diagnosed with BD1 vs. the population sample revealed significant differences involving four SNPs, rs2896635, rs2278749, rs969486 and rs2290035 and also another set of four SNPs in the 5′ region of the gene, namely rs2279287, rs1481892, rs7107287 and rs4757142. Neither TIMELESS nor PER3 showed significant differences among the samples for haplotype distributions.

SZ/SZA sample

The sample was composed of 331 cases (327 Caucasians, 4 African-Americans). The African-American probands were used in the family-based analyses only. There were 197 patients with SZ and 134 patients with SZA. Both parents were available for 149 cases. All Caucasian cases were used for comparisons against the neonatal sample (n = 327). Analyses were restricted to the SNPs analyzed in the NIMH BD1 samples (Table 2). Because similar patterns were observed in both the SZ and SZA families, they are presented together.

When we analyzed transmission bias in the SZ/SZA samples, we observed significant transmission distortion at loci in two genes, PER3 rs2859387 (χ2 = 5.36, df = 1, P = 0.020) and TIMELESS rs774026 (χ2 = 4.25, df = 1, P = 0.039). Contrasting genotype distributions in cases with the population-based sample did not reveal any significant differences by the trend test, although the genotype distribution for ARNTL rs1982350 was noted to be significantly different (χ2 = 7.137, df = 2, P = 0.028) (See Supplementary material).

To explore the bounds of the associated genomic region for each of three associated genes, we evaluated the transmission or distribution of haplotypes using the sliding window approach described previously. For ARNTL, no transmission bias was observed, although significant differences were observed overall for haplotypes bearing SNPs rs895682, rs1982350, rs2986635 and rs2290035 (data not shown). For PER3, transmission of haplotypes encompassing SNPs rs2859387, rs10462021 and rs4908699 showed significant bias (data not shown), but the contrast of haplotype distributions was not significant. Finally, for TIMELESS, transmission distortion was also observed for a haplotype based on SNPs rs774026 and rs2291738 (data not shown), the same haplotypes over-transmitted in the BD1 sample.

Discussion

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Materials and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments
  8. Supporting Information

Our study was designed to evaluate circadian gene variation in relation to BD1 liability. We evaluated several informative SNPs at each gene and analyzed two independent BD1 samples as well as a comparison SZ/SZA group. Family-based and case–control analyses were conducted at each SNP. We reasoned that such joint analysis would yield more credible results than either set of comparisons. Both the family-based analyses and the contrasts between the cases and the population-based sample suggested associations with ARNTL, PER3 and TIMELESS in the BD1 samples, albeit with different SNPs. Some of the SNPs are in significant linkage disequilibrium; hence, such associations are not independent. The differences between the family-based and case–control analyses may reflect variations in power (Bacanu et al. 2000). Overall, the magnitude of the associations is modest, and corrections for multiple comparisons were not applied. Replicate studies using larger samples are required.

As in the BD1 samples, associations were detected at PER3 and TIMELESS in the Pittsburgh SZ/SZA sample, using case–control comparisons and the TDT. However, the associated SNPs were different. The differences may be attributed to sample size variations or they may reflect differences in the architecture of liability conferred by the circadian genes. All these associations are modest and must be considered tentative pending replication. If replicated, our observations would lend credence to the possibility that certain genetic susceptibility factors are shared across the psychosis spectrum (Berrettini 2000).

We used an unscreened neonatal sample for contrast against our cases. As the lifetime prevalence of BD1 is approximately 1% (Kessler et al. 1994), the minimal loss of power incurred by including 1% false negatives is negligible (Bacanu et al. 2000). An unscreened sample from the same population is not expected to increase the rate of false positives.

In conclusion, the analysis of more than 1450 individuals suggested associations between three candidate circadian genes and liability for BD1. Other distinct associations were noted in SZ/SZA sample. None of these associations can be considered definitive, particularly because corrections for multiple comparisons were not applied. Nevertheless, the suggestive results warrant further investigations of the links between circadian variation and BD1 liability. Further studies using larger samples are in progress.

References

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Materials and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments
  8. Supporting Information
  • Abe, M. , Herzog, E.D. & Block, G.D. (2000) Lithium lengthens the circadian period of individual suprachiasmatic nucleus neurons. Neuroreport 11, 32613264.
  • Archer, S.N. , Robilliard, D.L. , Skene, D.J. , Smits, M. , Williams, A. , Arendt, J. & Von Schantz, M. (2003) A length polymorphism in the circadian clock gene Per3 is linked to delayed sleep phase syndrome and extreme diurnal preference. Sleep 26, 413415.
  • Bacanu, S.A. , Devlin, B. & Roeder, K. (2000) The power of genomic control. Am J Hum Genet 66, 19331944.
  • Baehr, E.K. , Revelle, W. & Eastman, C.I. (2000) Individual differences in the phase and amplitude of the human circadian temperature rhythm: with an emphasis on morningness-eveningness. J Sleep Res 9, 117127.
  • Basturk, M. , Karaaslan, F. , Esel, E. , Sofuoglu, S. , Tutus, A. & Yabanoglu, I. (2001) Effects of short and long-term lithium treatment on serum prolactin levels in patients with bipolar affective disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 25, 315322.DOI: 10.1016/S0278-5846(00)00165-2
  • Benedetti, F. , Serretti, A. , Colombo, C. , Barbini, B. , Lorenzi, C. , Campori, E. & Smeraldi, E. (2003) Influence of CLOCK gene polymorphism on circadian mood fluctuation and illness recurrence in bipolar depression. Am J Med Genet 123B, 2326.
  • Berrettini, W.H. (2000) Are schizophrenic and bipolar disorders related? A review of family and molecular studies. Biol Psychiatry 48, 531538.
  • Campbell, S.S. , Gillin, J.C. , Kripke, D.F. , Janowsky, D.S. & Risch, S.C. (1989) Lithium delays circadian phase of temperature and REM sleep in a bipolar depressive: a case report. Psychiatry Res 27, 2329.
  • Cermakian, N. & Boivin, D.B. (2003) A molecular perspective of human circadian rhythm disorders. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 42, 204220.
  • Chowdari, K.V. , Mirnics, K. , Semwal, P. , Wood, J. , Lawrence, E. , Bhatia, T. , Deshpande, S.N. , B, K.T. , Ferrell, R.E. , Middleton, F.A. , Devlin, B. , Levitt, P. , Lewis, D.A. & Nimgaonkar, V.L. (2002) Association and linkage analyses of RGS4 polymorphisms in schizophrenia. Hum Mol Genet 11, 13731380.
  • Clayton, D. (1999) A generalization of the transmission/disequilibrium test for uncertain-haplotype transmission. Am J Hum Genet 65, 11701177.
  • Desan, P.H. , Oren, D.A. , Malison, R. , Price, L.H. , Rosenbaum, J. , Smoller, J. , Charney, D.S. & Gelernter, J. (2000) Genetic polymorphism at the CLOCK gene locus and major depression. Am J Med Genet 96, 418421.DOI: 10.1002/1096-8628(20000612)96:3<418::AID-AJMG34>3.0.CO;2-S
  • Devlin, B. , Roeder, K. & Wasserman, L. (2001) Genomic control, a new approach to genetic-based association studies. Theor Popul Biol 60, 155166.DOI: 10.1006/tpbi.2001.1542
  • Ebisawa, T. , Uchiyama, M. , Kajimura, N. et al. (2001). Association of structural polymorphisms in the human period3 gene with delayed sleep phase syndrome. EMBO Rep 2, 342346.DOI: 10.1093/embo-reports/kve070
  • Ewens, W.J. and Spielman, R.S. (1995). The transmission/disequilibrium test: history, subdivision, and admixture. American Journal of Human Genetics 57, 45564.
  • Fallin, D. , Cohen, A. , Essioux, L. , Chumakov, I. , Blumenfeld, M. , Cohen, D. & Schork, N.J. (2001) Genetic analysis of case/control data using estimated haplotype frequencies: application to APOE locus variation and Alzheimer's disease. Genome Res 11, 143151.DOI: 10.1101/gr.148401
  • Healy, D. (1987) Rhythm and blues. Neurochemical, neuropharmacological and neuropsychological implications of a hypothesis of circadian rhythm dysfunction in the affective disorders. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 93, 271285.
  • Holzberg, D. & Albrecht, U. (2003) The circadian clock: a manager of biochemical processes within the organism. J Neuroendocrinol 15, 339343.DOI: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2003.00992.x
  • Horne, J.A. & Ostberg, O. (1976) A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 4, 97110.
  • Johansson, C. , Willeit, M. , Smedh, C. , Ekholm, J. , Paunio, T. , Kieseppa, T. , Lichtermann, D. , Praschak-Rieder, N. , Neumeister, A. , Nilsson, L.G. , Kasper, S. , Peltonen, L. , Adolfsson, R. , Schalling, M. & Partonen, T. (2003) Circadian clock-related polymorphisms in seasonal affective disorder and their relevance to diurnal preference. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 734739.DOI: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300121
  • Katz, G. , Durst, R. , Zislin, Y. , Barel, Y. & Knobler, H.Y. (2001) Psychiatric aspects of jet lag: review and hypothesis. Med Hypotheses 56, 2023.DOI: 10.1054/mehy.2000.1094
  • Katzenberg, D. , Young, T. , Finn, L. , Lin, L. , King, D.P. , Takahashi, J.S. & Mignot, E. (1998) A CLOCK polymorphism associated with human diurnal preference. Sleep 21, 569576.
  • Kennedy, S.H. , Kutcher, S.P. , Ralevski, E. & Brown, G.M. (1996) Nocturnal melatonin and 24-hour 6-sulphatoxymelatonin levels in various phases of bipolar affective disorder. Psychiatry Res 63, 219222.DOI: 10.1016/0165-1781(96)02910-1
  • Kessler, R.C. , McGonagle, K.A. , Zhao, S. , Nelson, C.B. , Hughes, M. , Eshleman, S. , Wittchen, H.U. & Kendler, K.S. (1994) Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51, 819.
  • Kripke, D.F. , Mullaney, D.J. , Atkinson, M. & Wolf, S. (1978) Circadian rhythm disorders in manic-depressives. Biol Psychiatry 13, 335351.
  • Kruglyak, L. (1999) Prospects for whole-genome linkage disequilibrium mapping of common disease genes. Nat Genet 22, 139144.DOI: 10.1038/9642
  • Lange, C. , Demeo, D. , Silverman, E.K. , Weiss, S.T. & Laird, N.M. (2004) PBAT: tools for family-based association studies. Am J Hum Genet 74, 367369.DOI: 10.1086/381563
  • Leibenluft, E. , Feldman-Naim, S. , Turner, E.H. , Schwartz, P.J. & Wehr, T.A. (1996) Salivary and plasma measures of dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) in patients with rapid cycling bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry 40, 731735.DOI: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00488-2
  • Manji, H.K. , Moore, G.J. & Chen, G. (2001) Bipolar disorder: leads from the molecular and cellular mechanisms of action of mood stabilizers. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 41, s107s119.DOI: 10.1192/bjp.178.41.s107
  • Mansour, H. , Wood, J. , Logue, T. , Chowdari, K.V. , Dayal, M. , Monk, T. , Kupfer, D. , Devlin, B. & Nimgaonkar, V. (2005) Circadian phase variation in Bipolar I Disorder (BD1). Chronobiol Int, in press.
  • Matsumoto, M. , Kamata, S. , Naoe, H. , Mutoh, F. & Chiba, S. (1996) [Investigation of the actual conditions of hospital nurses working on three rotating shifts: questionnaire results of shift work schedules, feelings of sleep and fatigue, and depression]. Seishin Shinkeigaku Zasshi 98, 1126.
  • Mellerup, E.T. , Lauritsen, B. , Dam, H. & Rafaelson, O.J. (1976) Lithium effects on diurnal rhythm of calcium, magnesium, and phosphate metabolism in manic-melancholic disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 53, 360370.
  • Mendlewicz, J. (1982) Circadian variation of serum TSH in unipolar and bipolar depression. Psychiatry Res 7, 388389.DOI: 10.1016/0165-1781(82)90076-2
  • Mendlewicz, J. , Linkowski, P. , Kerkhofs, M. , Leproult, R. , Copinschi, G. & Van Cauter, E. (1999) Genetic control of 24-hour growth hormone secretion in man: a twin study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 84, 856862.DOI: 10.1210/jc.84.3.856
  • Mitterauer, B. (2000) Clock genes, feedback loops and their possible role in the etiology of bipolar disorders: an integrative model. Med Hypotheses 55, 155159.DOI: 10.1054/mehy.1999.1039
  • Monk, T.H. , Reynolds, C.F.R. , Buysse, D.J. , Hoch, C.C. , Jarrett, D.B. , Jennings, J.R. & Kupfer, D.J. (1991) Circadian characteristics of healthy 80-year-olds and their relationship to objectively recorded sleep. J Gerontol 46, M171M175.
  • Nair, R.P. , Stuart, P. , Henseler, T. , Jenish, S. , Chia, N.V. , Westphal, E. , Schork, N.J. & E. (2000) Localization of psoriasis-susceptibility locus PSORS1 to a 60-kb interval telomeric to HLA-C. Am J Hum Genet 66, 18331844.DOI: 10.1086/302932
  • Nebel, L.E. , Howell, R.H. , Krantz, D.S. , Falconer, J.J. , Gottdiener, J.S. & Gabbay, F.H. (1996) The circadian variation of cardiovascular stress levels and reactivity: relationship to individual differences in morningness/eveningness. Psychophysiology 33, 273281.
  • Nurnberger, J.I.J.R. , Berrettini, W. , Simmons-Alling, S. , Lawrence, D. & Brittain, H. (1990) Blunted ACTH and cortisol response to afternoon tryptophan infusion in euthymic bipolar patients. Psychiatry Res 31, 5767.DOI: 10.1016/0165-1781(90)90109-I
  • Nurnberger, J.I.J.R. , Blehar, M.C. , Kaufmann, C.A. , York-Cooler, C. , Simpson, S.G. , Harkavy-Friedman, J. , Severe, J.B. , Malaspina, D. & Reich, T. (1994) Diagnostic interview for genetic studies. Rationale, unique features, and training. NIMH Genetics Initiative. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51, 849859; discussion 863–864.
  • O'Connell, J.R. & Weeks, D.E. (1998) PedCheck: a program for identification of genotype incompatibilties in linkage analysis. Am J Hum Genet 63, 259266.DOI: 10.1086/301904
  • Ranade, S.S. , Mansour, H. , Wood, J. , Chowdari, K.V. , Brar, L.K. , Kupfer, D.J. & Nimgaonkar, V.L. (2003) Linkage and association between serotonin 2A receptor gene polymorphisms and bipolar I disorder. Am J Med Genet 121B, 2834.DOI: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20070
  • Robilliard, D.L. , Archer, S.N. , Arendt, J. , Lockley, S.W. , Hack, L.M. , English, J. , Leger, D. , Smits, M.G. , Williams, A. , Skene, D.J. & Von Schantz, M. (2002) The 3111 Clock gene polymorphism is not associated with sleep and circadian rhythmicity in phenotypically characterized human subjects. J Sleep Res 11, 305312.DOI: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2002.00320.x
  • Sarai, K. & Kayano, M. (1968) The level and diurnal rhythm of serum serotonin in manic-depressive patients. Folia Psychiatr Neurol Jpn 22, 271281.
  • Schreiner, R. , Mirisch, S. , Vesely, Z. & Wiegand, M.H. (2001) Sleep and sleep-wake cycle in an 81-year-old patient with de novo ultra-rapid cycling bipolar disorder. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 251, 2931.DOI: 10.1007/s004060170064
  • Serretti, A. , Benedetti, F. , Mandelli, L. , Lorenzi, C. , Pirovano, A. , Colombo, C. & Smeraldi, E. (2003) Genetic dissection of psychopathological symptoms: insomnia in mood disorders and CLOCK gene polymorphism. Am J Med Genet 121B, 3538.DOI: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20053
  • Smith, C.S. , Reilly, C. & Midkiff, K. (1989) Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with suggestions for an improved measure of morningness. J Appl Psychol 74, 728738.DOI: 10.1037//0021-9010.74.5.728
  • Spielman, R.S. , McGinnis, R.E. & Ewens, W.J. (1993) Transmission test for linkage disequilibrium: the insulin gene region and insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus (IDDM). American Journal of Human Genetics 52, 50616.
  • Toh, K.L. , Jones, C.R. , He, Y. , Eide, E.J. , Hinz, W.A. , Virshup, D.M. , Ptacek, L.J. & Fu, Y.H. (2001) An hPer2 phosphorylation site mutation in familial advanced sleep phase syndrome. Science 291, 10401043.DOI: 10.1126/science.1057499
  • Vink, J.M. , Groot, A.S. , Kerkhof, G.A. & Boomsma, D.I. (2001) Genetic analysis of morningness and eveningness. Chronobiol Int 18, 809822.DOI: 10.1081/CBI-100107516

Acknowledgments

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Materials and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments
  8. Supporting Information

We thank the participants in our study. Supported in part by grants from the National Institutes of Health (MH63420, MH56242 and MH66263 to VLN; MH57881 to BD and AG13396 and AG020677 to THM), the Mental Health Intervention Research Center (MH 30915 to DJK) and NASA (NAG9-1234 to THM). We thank NIMH for providing DNA and data from the National Institute of Mental Health Collaborative Genetics Initiative (NIMH CGI) sample.

Additional details are available at our web site (http://www.pitt.edu/~nimga)

Supporting Information

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Materials and methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  7. Acknowledgments
  8. Supporting Information

Figure S1: LD patterns at ARNTL among parents of BDI patients. Figure S2: LD patterns at PER3 among parents of BDI patients. Figure S3: LD patterns at TIMELESS among parents of BDI patients. Figure S4: Trends test analyses at ARNTL and PER3. Table S1: SNPs analyzed. Table S2: Genomic sequences flanking the SNPs, and PCR primers. Table S3: Details of SNP Genotyping Assays. Table S4: Allele Frequencies of ANPs analyzed. Table S5: Case-control analysis at ARNTL, TIMELESS and PERIOD3.

FilenameFormatSizeDescription
GBB147FigureS1.doc652KSupporting info item
GBB147FigureS2.doc489KSupporting info item
GBB147FigureS3.doc182KSupporting info item
GBB147FigureS4.doc39KSupporting info item
GBB147TableS1.doc71KSupporting info item
GBB147TableS2.doc83KSupporting info item
GBB147TableS3.doc27KSupporting info item
GBB147TableS4.doc96KSupporting info item
GBB147TableS5.doc53KSupporting info item

Please note: Wiley Blackwell is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting information supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing content) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.