Unidirectional startle responses and disrupted left–right co-ordination of motor behaviors in robo3 mutant zebrafish

Authors


M. Granato, Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine, Philadelphia, PA 19104-6058, USA. E-mail: granatom@mail.med.upenn.edu

Abstract

The Roundabout (Robo) family of receptors and their Slit ligands play well-established roles in axonal guidance, including in humans where horizontal gaze palsy with progressive scoliosis (HGPPS) is caused by mutations in the robo3 gene. Although significant progress has been made toward understanding the mechanism by which Robo receptors establish commissural projections in the central nervous system, less is known about how these projections contribute to neural circuits mediating behavior. In this study, we report cloning of the zebrafish behavioral mutant twitch twice and show that twitch twice encodes robo3. We show that in mutant hindbrains the axons of an identified pair of neurons, the Mauthner cells, fail to cross the midline. The Mauthner neurons are essential for the startle response, and in twitch twice/robo3 mutants misguidance of the Mauthner axons results in a unidirectional startle response. Moreover, we show that twitch twice mutants exhibit normal visual acuity but display defects in horizontal eye movements, suggesting a specific and critical role for twitch twice/robo3 in sensory-guided behavior.

Bilateral symmetry is a property of the vertebrate nervous system which poses challenges for the processing of sensory stimuli and motor control. Commissural fibers play a crucial role in transferring sensory information across the midline and ensuring co-ordinated motor responses. During development, commissural axons are initially attracted to the ventral midline, then cross to the contralateral side and are repelled from the midline. This switch in growth cone responsiveness allows growth cones to use intermediate targets to which they are initially attracted but then must leave to continue toward their synaptic targets (Dickson 2002; Tessier-Lavigne & Goodman 1996). DCC receptors on commissural growth cones mediate the initial attraction toward Netrin producing midline cells, whereas levels of Roundabout (Robo) receptors remain low to minimize transmission of repulsive signals triggered by Slit ligands produced by midline cells (Kidd et al. 1998; Long et al. 2004). After midline crossing, growth cones express the Robo receptor, which suppresses signaling through the DCC receptor and makes the growth cone Slit responsive, thereby causing repulsion away from the midline (Kidd et al. 1998; Long et al. 2004). In the mouse, robo3 is required for axons to cross the midline (Sabatier et al. 2004), and analysis of alternative splice variants shows that Robo3.1 is critical for silencing Slit repulsion as growth cones approach the midline, whereas Robo3.2 acts after midline crossing with Robo1 and Robo2 to expel growth cones from the midline (Chen et al. 2008).

Much of our understanding of the role of Robo receptors in vertebrate neural circuit formation has been obtained from studies of commissural axon guidance. However, little is known about the behavioral function of neural circuits in which commissural neurons participate. This is in part because mice lacking Robo3 die a few hours after birth, precluding behavioral tests (Sabatier et al. 2004). However, mutations in the human robo3 gene cause horizontal gaze palsy with progressive scoliosis (HGPPS) (Jen et al. 2004), underlining the need for animal models in which to study the neurological consequences of loss of robo3 function.

In a genetic screen for genes required for assembling neural circuits involved in motor behavior, we identified a small group of mutants, including twitch twice, with no discernable morphological phenotype but which respond to tactile or vibrational stimuli by repeatedly performing turns toward the same side (Granato et al. 1996). In this study, we report on the molecular identification of the twitch twice gene and show that it encodes zebrafish robo3. twitch twice/robo3 null mutants display defects in the guidance of commissural hindbrain neurons, including the Mauthner neuron. We show that these guidance defects cause a unidirectional startle response and movement deficits specific to a subset of locomotor behaviors. Finally, twitch twice/robo3 mutants also exhibit specific defects in the co-ordination of horizontal eye movements. Thus, twitch twice/robo3 mutants provide a valuable model in which to examine neurological impairments in HGPPS as well as motor behavior deficits caused by the loss of Robo3 function.

Materials and methods

Fish maintenance

Zebrafish (Danio rerio) strains used in this study were twttw204 and twttx209 alleles of twitch twice, maintained on a mixed TLF and Tubingen background. The twttw204 and twttx209 mutant alleles appeared to be equally penetrant and were therefore both used for anatomical and behavioral characterization. Although about 10% of homozygotes develop swim bladders, all mutants die by 14 days post fertilization (dpf) presumably as the balance and motor deficits disrupt feeding. Enhancer or gene trap j1229a that expresses green fluorescence protein (GFP) in reticulospinal neurons including the Mauthner cells was identified in an enhancer/gene trap screen. The tol2 gene trap plasmid T2KSAG (Kawakami et al. 2004) was coinjected with tol2 transposase messenger RNA (mRNA). Embryos with any GFP positive cells were reared to adult stages, and their progeny were screened for specific expression patterns. Single insert lines were obtained by multiple generations of outcrossing and monitored by quantitative reverse transcriptase polymerase chain reaction. Once a single insert line was established, we used Tail-PCR (Parinov et al. 1999) to identify the genomic insertion site, at chr11:9,246,478 bp (ZV7 assembly). This location was confirmed by cosegregation of the reticulospinal expression pattern with PCR-based markers specific to the transposon in its insertion site (see http://www.genetics.wustl.edu/fish_lab/tol2/alleles/j1229a/j1229a.html). The j1229a insertion is not in or near an obvious gene, and thus the underlying molecular basis for its reticulospinal expression pattern is obscure.

Embryos were collected in the morning and thereafter maintained on a 14/10 h light/dark cycle at 28°C unless otherwise noted. Larvae were raised in 6-cm plastic Petri dishes at a density of 20–30 per 7 ml in E3 medium (5 mm NaCl, 0.17 mm KCl, 0.33 mm CaCl2, 0.33 mm MgSO4) with medium changes at 2 and 4 dpf. Behavioral experiments were conducted on 5–7 dpf.

Recombination mapping and molecular cloning

A three-generation mapping cross between twttw204 heterozygous and WIK fish was generated, and pools of 25 F2 mutant and 25 F2 sibling larvae were collected in the F2 generation and used for bulk segregant mapping. Mutant larvae were identified by visible abnormalities in the behavioral response to an acoustic or tactile startle stimulus, with larvae repeatedly bending to the same side, losing rhythmic left–right alternating tail movements (Granato et al. 1996) and failing to retain an upright posture throughout the response (see Results). For identifying the mutation, Robo3 cDNA was amplified in two fragments from 5 or 6 dpf larvae by RT-PCR, with primer design using Primer3 (Rozen & Skaletsky 2000). Reverse transcriptase-PCR was carried out using the Access RT-PCR kit (Promega, Madison, WI, USA) with 70 pmol of each primer and 500 ng of total mRNA prepared using TRIzol Reagent (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA) according to the manufacturer's protocol. For 5–6 dpf larvae, we used 20 μl TRIzol per larva. Control reactions containing no RNA or no reverse transcriptase were also performed. The primers for the 5′ fragment were 5′-TTTAGGGGACTGTTTGTCCG and 5′-AATCTCGTACTCGGTGCCTTT and the primers for the 3′ fragment were 5′-GTCGCAGTACATCC AGGGTT and 5′-TTCCGAATGAAAACAGAGGG. Reverse transcriptase-PCR conditions were 48°C for 45 min; 94°C for 2 min; and then five cycles at 94°C for 45 seconds, touchdown at 60°C to 55°C for 45 seconds, and 68°C for 150 seconds; and then 40 cycles at 94°C for 45 seconds, 55°C for 45 seconds, and 68°C for 150 seconds. Products were gel-purified and cloned into pGEM-T Easy (Promega, Madison, WI, USA) for sequencing. After comparing sequence derived from fragments cloned from mutant and sibling larvae, mutations were verified by repeating the RT-PCR on fresh pools of mutant and sibling larvae and directly sequencing gel-purified cDNA. All nucleotide numbers for robo3 refer to the Genbank mRNA sequence AF337036 (Robo3var1).

PCR genotyping twitch twice

We developed a dCAPS assay (Neff et al. 2002) using the dCAPS program (http://helix.wustl.edu/dcaps/dcaps.html) for genotyping both twitch twice alleles. For genotyping the twttx209 allele (C583T mutation), the primer pair is 5′-GAGCCTGCAACTTTGAACTG and 5′-CACACGTACACGCCTTCATC. Polymerase chain reaction is performed using 0.5-μm primers in RAPD buffer, using conditions: 94°C for 3 min, then three cycles at 94°C for 45 seconds, touchdown 60°C to 57°C for 45 seconds, 72°C for 15 seconds, then 37 cycles at 94°C for 45 seconds, 57°C for 45 seconds, 72°C for 15 seconds. The PCR product is then digested with FokI. The wild-type allele remains uncleaved at 194 bp, whereas the mutant allele is digested producing a 160-bp fragment which can be distinguished on a 3% agarose gel containing 2% Metaphor agarose (Lonza, Rockland, ME, USA). For genotyping the twttw204 allele (TC2099AA mutation), the primer pair is 5′-TGGTGGTCTATCAGCATCTCC and 5′-CGGAGATGTTTGCTGTGTAGTT . Polymerase chain reaction is performed as above and the product is digested with Eco57I. The wild-type allele is cleaved and contains a 160-bp fragment, whereas the mutant allele remains uncut at 194 bp. This protocol was used both for genotyping twitch twice adults and for genotyping larvae before the behavioral phenotype is manifest at 5 dpf.

Immunohistochemistry

To produce anti-Robo3 antisera, we subcloned a fragment of the robo3 cDNA encoding a 125 amino acid peptide predicted to be part of the Robo3 intracellular domain (amino acids 1142 to 1267, see Fig. 1c) into the PRSET vector (Invitrogen) in frame with a 6HIS tag. This was transformed in BL21DE3RIL bacteria (Stratagene, La Jolla, CA, USA), and protein expression was induced by 1.0 mm IPTG. Bacteria were pelleted and resuspended in 10 mm Tris pH 8.0, 100 mm NaH2PO4, 8 m urea for lysis. Tagged protein was then purified on a NiNTA column using the manufacturer's denaturing protocol. Fractions containing the expressed protein were dialyzed into 4 m urea. The purified protein fragment was then used to immunize rabbits (Prosci Inc., Poway, CA, USA). Bleeds were tested using wholemount immunostaining and Western blot from protein extracts of 24 hours post fertilization larvae. One bleed showed faint signs of reactivity, but high background. We therefore coupled the 6HIS tagged peptide to an Affigel-10 (BioRad, Hercules, CA, USA) immunoaffinity column using the manufacturer's protocol and absorbed the antisera to the column. We eluted with 0.1 m glycine pH 2.5 and pooled the most concentrated fractions for immunostaining. Purified anti-Robo3 was diluted 1:100 in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) and pre-absorbed to fixed embryos at 4°C overnight. For immunostaining, 10% normal goat serum and 0.5% Triton X-100 were added to pre-absorbed antibody. Larvae were kept for 3 h at 4°C in 4% paraformaldehyde with 1% dimethyl sulfoxide, treated with 1 mg/ml collagenase for 6 min and blocked for 30 min with 10% fetal bovine serum in PBS. Other antibodies used were GFP JL-8 (1:100 dilution, Clontech, Mountain View, CA, USA) and 3A10 [1:50, Hatta (1992)], kind gift of Dr T. Jessell). Primary antibodies were detected by AlexaFluor488- or AlexaFluor594-conjugated secondary antibodies (1:200, Invitrogen) or biotinylated anti-mouse antibody with the color reaction performed using the Vectorstain ABC kit (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA). Images were acquired with a Leica LCS confocal microscope and a Zeiss Axioplan microscope.

Figure 1.

Positional Cloning and Sequence Analysis of robo3. (a) Recombination mapping placed twitch twice between Z-markers z9574 and z531 on linkage group 10. (b) Sequence analysis of cDNA from robo3 showed nonsense mutations in both the tw204 and tx209 alleles of twitch twice. (c) Domain structure of robo3 showing the predicted truncated proteins generated by the mutations. The tx209 allele is predicted to truncate robo3 at amino acid R58 in the first immunoglobulin domain (green). The tw204 allele truncates the protein at F563 in the first fibronectin domain (orange). Also shown is the transmembrane domain (purple), conserved Roundabout motif boxes (red) and region used to make the antibody (blue).

Behavioral assays

All behavioral measurements were made with the Flote software package (Burgess & Granato 2007a,b). Briefly, this software performs tracking of larvae filmed in groups of up to 30 larvae, and then performs automated analysis of body curvature on each larva to extract kinematic details of swimming movements. Discrete locomotor movements are identified using kinematic features. In all experiments, larvae were pre-adapted to the intensity of light in the testing arena for at least 3 h. Video recordings were initiated 2 min after moving larvae to the testing arena in order to allow sufficient time for locomotor activity to stabilize (Burgess & Granato 2007a). All video recordings were made with a Motionpro high speed camera (Redlake, Tucson, AZ, USA) 512 × 512 pixel resolution, using a 50-mm macro lens at 1000 frames per second unless otherwise noted. Experiments were carried out at 26–28°C.

Behavioral analysis of twitch twice larvae is complicated by the frequency with which they fail to assume a vertical orientation in the water, instead falling toward one side. When filmed from above, the initial flexion of the body is therefore toward or away from the camera, not detectable with this recording configuration. However, larvae that fail to maintain a vertical orientation show only a single eye to the camera (Fig. 4a), allowing us to exclude these larvae from analysis. In addition, larvae already engaged in locomotion at the beginning of a video recording were excluded from analysis, as were larvae initiating movement in the last 20 ms, as insufficient frames remain to determine the type of motor pattern being initiated.

Figure 4.

Startle responses are unidirectional in twitch twice/robo3 mutants with Mauthner crossing defects. (a) Although wild-type sibling larvae remain vertically oriented in the water column, twitch twice mutants frequently lie on their side at the bottom of the dish and can be automatically recognized by the analysis software as only one eye is visible to the camera. (b) In stationary larvae, measuring the ‘single eye’ phenotype as a surrogate for horizontal posture shows that wild-type sibling fish very rarely adopt a horizontal posture, whereas twttw204 mutant larvae frequently lie on their side indicating a balance defect (n = 5 groups each). (c) Twitch twice/robo3 mutants roll during startle responses. Wild-type sibling larvae remained vertically oriented for at least 90% of the recording window. Such responses are classified as ‘Upright’. In contrast, twttw204 mutants show a strong tendency to roll during their startle response, with less than 60% of responses being categorized as Upright (n = 5 groups each). (d) SLC startle responses to a series of 40 acoustic stimuli at two intensities (presented in a pseudorandom order) are elicited with similar frequency in sibling and twttw204 mutants (n = 5 groups each). (e) LLC startle responses are elicited with lower frequency in sibling and twttw204 mutants. (f) SLC startle direction was assessed as the proportion of trials in which responses were initiated in a rightward direction. twttw204 larvae were presorted according to Mauthner axon phenotype (schematically depicted, n = 10 larvae each set) before testing. (g) LLC startle direction was assessed as for (f). *t-test P < 0.01; #one-sample t-test against 50% P < 0.001.

Stationary balance was measured by taking a 1-min video (1 frame per second), with illumination provided by an array of 80 infrared LEDs (940 nm peak; ILED-8; AllElectronics, Van Nuys, CA, USA), mounted 75 mm below the testing arena. A dorsally positioned light was provided (300 μW/cm2), although it is not clear that young larvae actually use this information for vertical orientation. Larvae with balance deficits were detected by their horizontal posture as described above. For each group, the percentage of observations of larvae with a horizontal posture was calculated.

Dark Flash responses were measured by pre-adapting larvae to 300 μW/cm2 white light before moving them to the testing arena. In the testing arena, illumination was provided by an array of six white LEDs (Jameco Electronics, 320531, Belmont, CA, USA) adjusted to 300 μW/cm2 using GamColor neutral density filters (GAM, Los Angeles, CA, USA) and controlled by a Stamp BS2sx microcontroller (Parallax, Rocklin, CA, USA). The microcontroller simultaneously triggered the camera to start recording a 1000 frame burst coincident with turning the light off for 1 second (a ‘Dark Flash’). Larvae were tested in groups of 20 per plate, with each group subjected to a series of eight Dark Flashes presented at 1-min intervals.

Startle responses were elicited using either of two techniques. To measure startle responsiveness, kinematics and balance, larvae were tested in groups of 20, using a small vibration exciter (4810, Bruel and Kjaer, Norcross, GA, USA), controlled by an digital-analogue card (PCI-6221, National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA), which also triggered the camera to collect a 120 frame (120 ms) window. All startle stimuli were 3-ms duration, 1000 Hz waveforms, of variable intensity as specified in the text, generated by custom software. Bend angle, duration and maximal angular velocity are as previously defined (Burgess & Granato 2007a). Locomotor balance was measured by tracking the position of the eyes for each larva during every frame of its response to the stimulus. Wild-type larvae remain vertically oriented for at least 90% of frames even during the vigorous movements made in response to startle stimuli. Thus, responses are scored as balanced only if both eyes can be identified in >90% of frames. In experiments in which startle directionality was assessed, an impact ‘tap’ stimulus was delivered by a tubular solenoid (S-63-38-H, Magnetic Sensor Systems, Van Nuys, CA, USA), whose activation was co-ordinated with the video camera using a Stamp microcontroller. Startle-inducing stimuli were delivered at 15-second intervals to minimize habituation. We excluded larvae that were horizontally oriented at the beginning of the recording window from further analysis, as these larvae tend to lie against the bottom of the dish and may receive a tactile stimulus in addition to the acoustic stimulus. In addition, we cannot effectively analyze kinematics or direction of movement in these larvae.

Optokinetic responses (OKR) were elicited using a DLP projector (PG-M20x; Sharp, Mahwah, NJ, USA) following the protocol of Huang and Neuhauss (2008). Larvae were embedded in 50 μl of 3% methylcellulose on a 3-cm Petri dish with one eye facing a cylindrical diffusion screen at a distance of 12 cm and the other eye facing a black piece of paper. Illumination was provided with an infrared array as described above. Using the DLP projector, a vertical step grating (0.053 cycles/degree spatial frequency) was moved in either a nasal to temporal (NT) direction, or the reverse, at a constant velocity of either 8.3 degree/second or 16.7 degree/second for 4 seconds. The grating was corrected for the curvature of screen so that the spatial frequency remained constant across the screen. The intensity of the grating stepped from 115 to 0.34 μW/cm2. Each larva was presented with three stimuli for each combination of direction and velocity (12 trials total). Software for the stimulus was written in IDL (ITT Visual Information Solutions, Boulder, CO, USA). Video recordings were made at 100 frames per second, with the start synchronized to the initiation of the test using a microcontroller and a photodiode. Code for analysis of eye velocity was written in IDL. First, a bandpass filter was applied and local maxima in the field (corresponding to eye centroids) found. Pixels of similar intensity to the centroid were located and their position used as input to the Fit_Ellipse routine (courtesy of David Fanning, www.dfanning.com) to find the orientation of the major axis of the fitted ellipse. The orientation time series was smoothed using a boxcar function (width 21 points), and the angular velocity of eye rotations was measured by finding the gradient of the linear fit to the interval between the end of the first second of stimulation and the first saccade (if any). Optokinetic gain was calculated as the ratio of the angular velocity of eye movement to the angular velocity of the stimulus. The relatively low gains reported here are consistent with results derived by other groups using monocular stimulation in larval zebrafish (Beck et al. 2004; Qian et al. 2005). Circuit diagrams and software will be provided upon request.

Statistical analyses

All statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 14.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). Where appropriate, P values were subjected to Bonferroni correction to maintain alpha of 0.05. Graphs show mean and standard error except where otherwise noted.

Results

Molecular cloning of twitch twice

Using recombination mapping, we mapped the twitch twice locus to a 1.7 cM interval between two simple sequence length polymorphic markers, z531 (4/254 recombinants) and z9574 (6/248 recombinants) on chromosome 10 (Fig. 1a). Several mRNAs and ESTs had previously been mapped to this interval, including Robo3. Sequencing of robo3 cDNAs isolated from twttw204 mutant larvae showed two adjacent nucleotide changes, T2099A and C2100A, introducing a premature stop codon predicted to truncate the protein in the first fibronectin repeat of the extracellular domain (Fig. 1b,c). We then amplified and sequenced cDNAs from the twttx209 allele and found a C583T nonsense mutation, predicted to truncate Robo3 in the first immunoglobin repeat at amino acid 58, thereby generating a presumptive null allele (Fig. 1b,c). Thus, sequence analysis identified premature stop codons in two independent twitch twice alleles, showing that the twitch twice phenotype is caused by mutations in the robo3 gene.

Mauthner cell pathfinding defects in twitch twice/robo3

Previous studies showed that zebrafish Robo3 is expressed in regions of the hindbrain consistent with the position of differentiating reticulospinal neurons (Challa et al. 2001, 2005; Lee et al. 2001). In mice lacking Robo3, commissural spinal cord and hindbrain axons fail to cross the midline because they become prematurely Slit responsive (Sabatier et al. 2004). To determine the role of twitch twice/robo3 in zebrafish midline guidance, we examined the axonal trajectory of identified hindbrain neurons, the Mauthner cells, a pair of large reticulospinal neurons which project to the contralateral spinal cord starting at 17 hpf (Fig. 2a). Immunohistochemistry with the 3A10 antibody showed a dramatic Mauthner cell pathfinding defect in a subset of mutants. In these larvae, one Mauthner axon, instead of traversing the ventral midline, projects ipsilaterally to the spinal cord (Fig. 2b). In rare instances, both Mauthner axons project ipsilaterally to the spinal cord (Fig. 2c). No instances of Mauthner pathfinding defects were observed in wild-type sibling larvae (n = 312). The Mauthner pathfinding defect in twitch twice/robo3 mutants is temperature sensitive, with 48% (73/153) of the larvae displaying the phenotype when raised at 22°C for the first 24 h post fertilization, dropping to 19% (13/68) when raised at 28°C over the same period (Fisher's exact test P < 0.001). At 24°C, 37.3% of larvae have a single Mauthner axon defect and 10.5% have a double ipsilateral projection, consistent with the probability of a misprojection being independent for the two cells. Commissural pathfinding defects are not restricted to Mauthner axons. In wild-type sibling larvae, the 3A10 antibody shows a ladder of seven commissural tracts in the caudal hindbrain (Lorent et al. 2001) (Fig. 2d). In twitch twice/robo3 larvae, the hindbrain commissures are disorganized, defasciculated and frequently merge. However, unlike in space cadet mutants (Lorent et al. 2001), the morphology of the axon cap is normal (Fig. 2e).

Figure 2.

Mauthner axons fail to cross the midline in twitch twice/robo3. Immunostaining with the 3A10 antibody in 48 hpf embryos highlights the Mauthner cell and axons. In wild-type sibling embryos (a) both Mauthner axons cross the midline and project caudally. In twitch twice/robo3 mutants (b,c) one or both Mauthner axons fail to cross the midline and project to ipsilateral spinal cord. At 6 dpf, immunostaining with the 3A10 antibody shows a characteristic ladder of commissures (white triangles) and the axon cap of the Mauthner cell (black triangles) in wild-type larvae (d). Commissures are disorganized in twitch twice/robo3 mutants although the Mauthner axon cap is intact (e).

robo3 expression in commissural axons

To examine the expression of Robo3 protein as commissural axons cross the midline, we raised polyclonal antibodies against zebrafish Robo3. Immunohistochemical staining with this antibody shows a ladder of commissures in the hindbrain of 48 hpf wild-type sibling larvae (Fig. 3a). Commissure staining is absent in twitch twice mutants, showing the specificity of the antibody for Robo3 (Fig. 3b). Next we examined wild-type larvae at 18 hpf, when Mauthner axons approach the ventral midline. In order to identify Mauthner axons, we used a transgenic line with early GFP expression in the Mauthner cell (Miyashita et al. 2004). Double staining with anti-GFP and anti-Robo3 antibodies showed accumulation of Robo3 protein on the growth cones of Mauthner cells before they reached the ventral midline (Fig. 3c). This is consistent with the role of Robo3 in mouse, where expression on precrossing commissural axons renders them insensitive to Slit, thereby enabling these growth cones to approach and cross the ventral midline.

Figure 3.

robo3 is expressed in commissural axons. (a,b) Co-immunostaining of 48 hpf larvae was performed with anti- robo3 (green) and the 3A10 antibody (red) to show the Mauthner axon phenotype in wild-type sibling larvae (a) and twitch twice/robo3 mutants (b). (c) robo3 expression in precrossing growth cones was analyzed by fixing actin:GFP transgenic fish at 10-min intervals starting at 18 somites. Anti-GFP (green) and anti- robo3 staining co-localize showing that robo3 is expressed before Mauthner axons have crossed the midline.

Startle responses in twitch twice/robo3 mutant larvae

In fish, the Mauthner cell plays a central role in controlling startle responses (Eaton et al. 1981; Kimmel et al. 1980; Liu & Fetcho 1999). We therefore examined the startle response in twitch twice/robo3 mutant larvae. We noticed that although normal larvae remained vertically oriented in the water column, twitch twice/robo3 larvae often lay on their side at the bottom of the dish when stationary and frequently rolled over during the startle response, suggesting a balance impairment (Fig. 4a). Measurement of this phenotype showed that mutant larvae show a significantly higher incidence of poor balance in the absence of stimulation (n = 5 groups each genotype, t-test P = 0.006; Fig. 4b). Wild-type sibling larvae maintain a vertical posture throughout the startle response [supporting information, movie S1(a)]. In contrast, mutant larvae roll during the course of the response [supporting information, movie S1(b)]. As wild-type larvae occasionally tilt briefly during the startle response, we set a threshold for normal responses as being those where the fish remained upright for more than 90% of the frames analyzed. Based on this criterion, 94.6% ± 0.4% of wild-type sibling startle responses were upright compared with only 55.6% ± 1.7% of mutant startles (n = 5 groups per phenotype, t-test P = 0.003; Fig. 4c). Wild-type larvae respond to a sudden decrease in illumination (Dark Flash) by performing a slow turn in which the head and tail of the larvae almost touch (O-bend), followed by a counterbend and bout of swimming (Burgess & Granato 2007a). Mutants are also unstable during responses to Dark Flash stimuli with only 68.5% ± 6.9% of mutant O-bends maintaining an upright posture compared with 96.2% ± 1.9% of responses in wild-type siblings (n = 5 groups per phenotype, t-test P = 0.01). Thus, twitch twice/robo3 larvae have impaired balance, leading to abnormal rolling during vigorous locomotor responses.

We have previously shown that zebrafish larvae show two stereotyped modes of response to acoustic startle stimuli (Burgess & Granato 2007b). One mode of response occurs at very short latency to the stimulus (<15 ms) and is initiated with an explosive C-bend of the body (short latency C-start or ‘SLC’ response). The second type of response occurs at a longer latency (mean 28 ms) and has less vigorous kinematic properties (long latency C-start or ‘LLC’ response). Moreover, SLC responses are the classic Mauthner-mediated startle response of teleost fish, whereas LLC responses are Mauthner independent. To determine whether SLC and/or LLC behavior depend on the neural circuitry established by Robo3 guidance, we tested responsiveness to acoustic/vibrational stimuli. twitch twice/robo3 mutants showed almost identical SLC responsiveness when compared with wild-type sibling larvae [two-way analysis of variance (anova), no effect of genotype F[1,16] = 2.1, P = 0.16; Fig. 4d]. However, mutants showed a quantitative reduction in LLC responsiveness (two-way anova, main effect of genotype F[1,16] = 7.9, P = 0.012; Fig. 4e). Long latency C-start responses are mediated by the otoliths of the inner ear which are also required for balance. Otoliths are normal in twitch twice/robo3 fish, thus the impairment in both balance and LLC responsiveness suggests that stato-acoustic afferents are impaired in twitch twice/robo3.

In twitch twice/robo3 mutants, one of the two Mauthner axons frequently fails to cross the midline and instead projects on the ipsilateral side to the spinal cord. Thus, one side of the spinal cord receives a double innervation of Mauthner axons, whereas the other side receives no such input. We therefore hypothesized that in such single ipsilateral twitch twice mutants, SLC responses should be selectively lateralized. To test this, we first sorted larvae according to their Mauthner cell phenotype, using the 1229a enhancer trap line with GFP expression in 4 dpf Mauthner cells and axons (data not shown). Individual larvae were then tested with a series of acoustic/vibrational stimuli delivered to the side of the Petri dish by a solenoid (Burgess & Granato 2007b). Wild-type larvae and twitch twice/robo3 mutants in which both Mauthner axons crossed the midline showed no directional bias to SLC responses with close to 50% of responses being made in a rightward direction (Fig. 4f). In contrast, twitch twice/robo3 mutants in which the left Mauthner axon projected together with the right Mauthner axon to the left spinal cord showed almost exclusively leftward SLC responses (3.3% ± 1.7% of responses were rightward, one-sample t-test vs. 50%, P < 0.001). Similarly, in mutants where the right Mauthner axon projected ipsilaterally, SLC responses were almost entirely rightward (98.6% ± 1.4% of responses rightward, one-sample t-test vs. 50%, P < 0.001). In the rare mutants where both Mauthner axons failed to cross, SLC responses showed no directional bias, reflecting the fact that both sides of the spinal cord received Mauthner innervation. Confirming our previous data showing that LLC responses do not require the Mauthner cell, these responses were initiated leftward and rightward with equal frequency in all groups (Fig. 4g). Thus, miswiring of the neural circuit mediating startle responses in twitch twice/robo3 mutants specifically impairs the ability of the fish to generate SLC responses in both left and right directions.

Kinematic defects in twitch twice/robo3

To examine in more detail the behavioral defects observed in twitch twice/robo3 mutants, we analyzed the kinematic properties of the first C-bend and counterbend of responses made to acoustic and visual stimuli (Table 1). The kinematic properties of SLC startle responses made by twitch twice/robo3 mutants to acoustic stimuli were indistinguishable from wild-type sibling larvae (Fig. 5a; Table 1). In contrast, LLC responses to acoustic startle stimuli were characterized by an extended duration of the first C-bend and reduced angular velocity of the counterbend such that larvae appear to perform a single ‘twitch’ in response to the stimulus (Fig. 5b; Table 1). Mutant larvae show similar responsiveness to Dark Flash stimuli (siblings 93.2% ± 2.7%, mutants 84.8% ± 3.5%; n = 5 groups each; t-test P = 0.092). However, O-bend responses to Dark Flash stimuli were affected in a similar fashion to LLC responses in that the duration of the initial C-bend was greatly extended and the counterbend performed at reduced angular velocity (Fig. 5c; Table 1). In addition, the angular velocity of the initial C-bend was reduced. As for LLC responses, the effect was to produce a ‘twitch’ response to the stimulus [supporting information, movie S2(a,b)]. The lack of impairment in the kinematics of SLC responses suggests that muscle function is not impaired in mutants and that ‘long slow’ kinematics of LLC and O-bend responses reflect deficits in neural circuits generating these responses.

Table 1.  C-bend kinematics for acoustic startle and dark flash responses in twitch twice/robo3
inline image
Figure 5.

C-bend kinematics are disrupted in twitch twice/robo3 mutant LLC and O-bend responses. Larval curvature is plotted for wild-type sibling (black, n = 18–20) and twttw204 (gray, n = 18–20) responses. Response latencies vary, so curves were manually inspected and shifted so that the beginning of each response is at the same position on the x-axis. For SLC (a) and LLC (b) responses to an acoustic stimulus, a 50-ms interval is shown, whereas a 100-ms interval is shown for the O-bend response to a Dark Flash stimulus to accommodate the longer duration bend and counterbend (c).

Failure to co-ordinate horizontal eye movements in twitch twice/robo3

The robo3 gene has been implicated in the human neurological disorder HGPPS (Jen et al. 2004). In HGPPS, patients show a near complete absence of all horizontal gaze reflexes (Bosley et al. 2005). We therefore asked whether zebrafish robo3 mutants recapitulated this phenotype. Horizontal eye movements can be evoked in zebrafish larvae by eliciting the OKR to a horizontally moving grating (Fig. 6a) (Clark 1981; Easter & Nicola 1997).

Figure 6.

Horizontal eye movements in twitch twice/robo3 mutants. (a) The optokinetic response was elicited by presenting a moving grating to one eye. Both the stimulated eye and the unstimulated eye rotate in the same direction as the stimulus. Angular velocity of eye movement in the stimulated eye (b) and unstimulated eye (c) was measured for low and high angular velocity stimuli for wild-type sibling (black, n = 15) and twttw204 mutant larvae (gray, n = 15). Negative angular velocity trials represent the stimulus moving in a nasal to temporal direction, whereas positive angular velocity represents a temporal to nasal stimulus. *P < 0.05.

First, we tested the response of the eye facing the optokinetic stimulus. When tested with a moving grating moving from the temporal to nasal direction, twitch twice/robo3 mutants and siblings showed an almost identical ability to track the grating under both low and high angular velocity stimulus conditions (two-way anova, no effect of genotype F[1,56] = 0.22, P = 0.64; main effect of stimulus velocity F[1,56] = 4.3, P = 0.044; Fig. 6b). Thus, similar to the situation in patients with HGPPS, horizontal eye movements are not overly impaired in zebrafish robo3 mutants. Lateral eyed, afoveate animals including zebrafish less effectively track stimuli moving in the NT direction (Qian et al. 2005; Roeser & Baier 2003). Consistent with this, we found that in wild-type sibling fish the optokinetic gain for NT stimuli was reduced by 50% when compared with temporal to nasal stimulation. In contrast, gain was reduced by almost 90% in twitch twice/robo3 mutants such that mutants showed a significantly lower gain than siblings for NT stimuli (main effect of genotype, F[1,56] = 55.7, P < 0.001; Fig. 6b).

Next, we examined movements of the unstimulated eye during the OKR. Stimulation of one eye during the OKR triggers lower gain but same direction movements by the unstimulated eye (Qian et al. 2005; Rinner et al. 2005). We found that co-ordinated rotation of the unstimulated eye was greatly reduced in mutants compared with sibling larvae (anova, main effect of genotype F[1,56] = 10.9, P = 0.002; Fig. 6c). As the stimulated eye has a normal OKR response, it is unlikely that twitch twice/robo3 has reduced visual acuity or a motor deficit. Rather, the poor response of the unstimulated eye likely represents a failure to transmit signals for motor control to the opposite side of the brain.

Discussion

In this study, we show that two alleles of the twitch twice mutant contain nonsense mutations in the robo3 gene. One allele of twitch twice/robo3 harbors a rare double mutation in neighboring base pairs. Although ENU usually induces single nucleotide mutations, changes in adjacent base pairs have been reported (Douglas et al. 1995). Mutations in both alleles are predicted to truncate Robo3 in the extracellular domain thereby acting as null alleles. We show that robo3 is required for midline guidance of commissural axons in zebrafish and show that miswiring of Mauthner cell projections leads to unidirectional initiation of the startle response and defects in the co-ordinated expression of other motor behaviors.

As in mammals, four Robo receptors have been identified in zebrafish (Challa et al. 2001; Lee et al. 2001; Park et al. 2003). The zebrafish robo2/astray gene is critical for guidance of retinal ganglion cell and olfactory axons (Fricke et al. 2001; Miyasaka et al. 2005). Both N-terminal and C-terminal alternative splice variants of the mouse robo3 gene have been described, with amino terminal splice products showing differential Slit binding (Camurri et al. 2005; Chen et al. 2008; Mambetisaeva et al. 2005; Sabatier et al. 2004). Similarly, the zebrafish robo3 gene produces two isoforms, which vary in their amino termini. Robo3var1 is predominantly expressed in non-neuronal cells (somites, fin bud mesenchyme), and morpholino-mediated knockdown of Robo3var1 causes defects in dorsoventral pattering of the early embryo (Challa et al. 2005). Robo3var2 is more restricted to neural tissues (hindbrain, spinal cord) (Challa et al. 2005). The mutations in twitch twice affect both Robo3var1 and Robo3var2; however, maternal transcripts of robo3 are present in zebrafish (Challa et al. 2005; Burgess and Granato, data not shown), and may compensate for the loss of zygotic protein, explaining the partial penetrance of the Mauthner axon decussation defect in twitch twice/robo3 mutants.

Robo3 is a divergent member of the Robo family (Challa et al. 2001; Lee et al. 2001), which shares only partial sequence conservation of the CC1 and CC2 motifs, necessary for signaling via DCC and Ena (Bashaw et al. 2000; Stein & Tessier-Lavigne 2001). The Drosophila Robo2 protein, which also lacks the CC2 motif, appears to mediate a weaker response to midline Slit and through heterodimerization, may inhibit Robo signaling (Rajagopalan et al. 2000; Simpson et al. 2000). This suggests that zebrafish Robo3 may also function to block signaling through Robo to facilitate commissural axon extension toward the midline despite the presence of Slit. Consistent with this, we find that Robo3 protein is expressed on the growth cones of Mauthner cells extending toward the ventral midline. Similarly, in the mouse, the Robo3.1 isoform is expressed and functions as commissural axons approach and cross the midline to antagonize midline Slit repulsion (Chen et al. 2008).

In humans, mutations in the robo3 gene cause HGPPS. Although we did not observe scoliosis in young larvae, this likely reflects the relatively late onset and progressive nature of scoliosis in patients (Bosley et al. 2005; Dretakis & Kondoyannis 1974). It was not possible to examine older animals as homozygous mutant fish die by 14 dpf. Although the neurological basis of horizontal gaze palsy in HGPPS remains unclear, patients show a loss of brainstem commissural tracts and midline crossing defects in long ascending medial lemniscal and descending corticospinal tracts (Bosley et al. 2005; Sicotte et al. 2006). In consequence, ipsilateral motor cortex is activated during motor tasks where contralateral activation is seen in normals (Haller et al. 2008). Our finding that robo3 mutations also cause commissural fiber defects in twitch twice represents an opportunity to analyze circuit level defects that contribute to the neuropathology of this disorder.

Behavioral deficits in twitch twice/robo3 mutants do not reflect simple deficits in sensory acuity or motor function as fish show normal levels of sensitivity to acoustic and visual stimuli and the initial C-bend and counterbend of SLC startle responses are similar to wild-type. Rather, consistent with an essential role for commissural tracts in distributing sensory information to the two sides of the nervous system and facilitating co-ordinated motor output, we found that twitch twice/robo3 mutants show subtle behavioral defects in co-ordinating left and right motor responses to stimuli.

The most dramatic phenotype of twitch twice/robo3 larvae is the unidirectional initiation of short latency startle responses to acoustic stimuli in the subset of mutants with a unilateral Mauthner axon midline crossing defect. The teleost Mauthner cell has long been implicated in mediating startle responses (Kimmel et al. 1980; Liu & Fetcho 1999; Yasargil & Diamond 1968; Zottoli et al. 1999). Zebrafish larvae exhibit two distinct modes of response to an acoustic startle stimulus, a Mauthner-mediated explosive response initiated at short latency to the stimulus (SLC) and a long latency, less vigorous non-Mauthner response (LLC) (Burgess & Granato 2007b). In mutants in which one Mauthner cell fails to cross the midline, instead projecting with the other Mauthner cell to the ipsilateral spinal cord, SLC responses are initiated in one direction only, whereas LLC responses remain bidirectional. As twitch twice/robo3 larvae show a significant balance defect, we suspect that the rare instances in which responses are made to the side receiving no Mauthner input simply represent larvae positioned upside-down, a posture which cannot be detected in our images. Robo3 therefore plays a key role in ensuring that the neural circuit mediating the startle response is able to trigger SLC movements in both directions.

While the kinematics of SLC responses of mutant larvae are indistinguishable from wildtype siblings, we find that other types of stereotyped movement are impaired. The initial C-bend and counterbend of LLC responses to acoustic stimuli and O-bend responses to Dark Flash stimuli have reduced angular velocity and extended response duration. This resembles a weak ‘accordion’ phenotype characterized by defects in the normal alternation of contraction of the two sides of the tail musculature. In the bandoneon mutant, a mutation in the glycine receptor β-subunit disrupts reciprocal inhibition required to coordinate this process (Hirata et al. 2005). The kinematic defects in twitchtwice/robo3 could therefore reflect a loss of in reciprocal inhibition at the spinal level, or alternatively arise as a result of inappropriate bilateral activation of hindbrain locomotor circuits. Indeed Robo3 is expressed in both locations. However as the kinematics of SLC responses are spared in mutants, we suspect that spinal motor cicrcuits are relatively unimpaired. As we found extensive Robo3 protein expression in brainstem commissures, we propose that in mutants, disruption of reciprocal inhibition across these commissures allows both sides of the brainstem to be activated and hence bilateral activation of spinal motor circuits leading to an accordion-like phenotype. This may correspond to the intriguing finding that in HGPPS patients, left-sided monaural stimulation triggers bilateral activation of the primary auditory cortex rather than activation only of the contralateral side (Haller et al. 2008). Why then are SLC responses kinematically normal? Unlike LLC responses, SLC responses are mediated by the Mauthner cell. The fact that SLC responses do not show signs of bilateral activation may be a peculiarity of the fast escape circuitry which safeguards against simultaneous activation of both sides of the tail musculature by the two Mauthner cells during urgent escape responses. Activation of one Mauthner cell rapidly triggers feedback inhibition onto both Mauthner cells, preventing either neuron from initiating a second startle response while the first is ongoing. Moreover while the Mauthner cell directly activates contralateral motor neurons to initiate startle responses, it also indirectly inhibits ipsilateral motor neurons at extremely short latency, preventing the other Mauthner cell from activating a response (Faber & Korn 1978). Thus we propose that kinematic defects in LLC and O-Bend responses arise from simultaneous activation of the spinal cord due to defective crossed inhibition in the brainstem. SLC responses do not show kinematic defects because simultaneous Mauthner cell activation is prevented by multiple mechanisms which safeguard specifically against this contingency.

As the most pronounced neurological deficit in HGPPS is the near complete absence of horizontal gaze reflexes, we measured the optokinetic reflex to a horizontally moving grating in twitch twice/robo3 larvae. Optokinetic defects have been described in a variety of zebrafish mutants, including the belladonna mutant in which retinal ganglion cell axons fail to cross the midline and project to the ipsilateral brain (Rick et al. 2000). Achiasmatic mutants show reversed OKRs, thought to reflect ipsilateral target regions incorrectly interpreting the stimulus as having the reverse direction and driving compensatory eye movements accordingly. In contrast, in twitch twice retinal ganglion cell axons successfully decussate at the optic chiasm (data not shown), thus input to the central optokinetic system is likely normal, and indeed we did not find a significant difference in optokinetic gain when the stimulated eye was presented with a TN-directed stimulus. Surprisingly though, twitch twice/robo3 larvae show a markedly reduced response to optokinetic stimuli moving in the NT direction.

In frontal eyed, foveate animals, the OKR is primarily mediated by the cortex, with rapid eye movements in both TN and NT directions serving to maintain stability of the target image on the fovea (Harris et al. 1980). However, in lateral eyed, afoveate animals such as fish, optokinetic movements are produced by brainstem regions homologous to the nucleus of the optic tract (NOT) and are less efficient at tracking NT-directed stimuli [reviewed in Huang and Neuhauss (2008)]. Evidence from mammals suggests that each NOT is activated by TN directional stimuli at the contralateral eye and triggers horizontal rotation of both eyes away from the stimulated eye (Collewijn 1975; Mustari & Fuchs 1990). Thus, in order to drive eye rotation, monocular NT stimuli must activate the ipsilateral NOT by an indirect pathway, with information transferred back across the midline. The fact that NT stimuli inhibit the NOT (Hoffmann & Schoppmann 1975) and the presence of spontaneously active, likely GABAergic, neurons in the NOT which project to the contralateral NOT (Prochnow et al. 2007) suggests the hypothesis that NT stimuli activate the ipsilateral NOT by relieving reciprocal inhibition. Thus, the reduced response of twitch twice/robo3 mutants to NT stimuli would arise from defective midline crossing of the inhibitory connection.

The bilateral symmetry of the vertebrate brain plan imposes a need for robust lines of communication between the two sides of the brain. Our data show that the robo3 gene plays an important role in establishing the commissural tracts that allow the two halves of the brain to distribute sensory stimuli and co-ordinate motor responses in a manner required for normal behavior.

Acknowledgments

This work was funded by NRSA postdoctoral fellowship to H.A.B., National Institute of Health grant DK-069466 to S.L.J and National Institute of Health grants NS-048258 and MH-075691 to M.G.

Ancillary