SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

Keywords:

  • BDNF;
  • cognitive processing;
  • COMT;
  • genetics;
  • public service announcements

Abstract

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References

Anti-smoking public service announcements (PSAs) often use persuasive arguments to attempt to influence attitudes about smoking. The persuasiveness of a PSA has previously been associated with factors that influence the cognitive processing of its message. Genetic factors that influence cognitive processing might thus affect individuals' responses to the persuasive arguments presented in PSAs. In the present study, we examined polymorphisms in the genes encoding brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF Val66Met) and catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT Val158Met), which affect cognitive processing in the prefrontal cortex, to identify genetic factors associated with self-reported outcomes of message processing, perceived effectiveness and quitting intentions among smokers viewing PSAs. A total of 120 smokers viewed sets of four PSAs that varied with respect to features of argument strength (AS) and message sensation value. We observed significant associations of BDNF genotype with central processing, narrative processing, perceived effectiveness of the anti-smoking PSAs and participant quitting intentions; the BDNF Met allele was associated with lower scores on all these measures. Central processing acted as a mediator of the association of genotype with quitting intentions and perceived effectiveness. There was a significant interaction of COMT genotype by AS in the model of narrative processing, such that individuals homozygous for the COMT Val allele reported higher narrative processing in the high-AS condition but not in the low-AS condition. To our knowledge, this is the first study to identify genetic factors associated with cognitive processing of anti-smoking PSAs.

Tobacco smoking is the leading preventable cause of death worldwide (World Health Organization 2009). Anti-smoking advertising campaigns using public service announcements (PSAs) have been employed in efforts to decrease smoking prevalence (Biener et al. 2000; Messer et al. 2007; Siegel 1998). Anti-smoking PSAs often use persuasive arguments to influence public attitudes about smoking, and features affecting cognitive processing of the arguments influence the perceived persuasiveness of PSAs (Biener et al. 2000; Leshner & Cheng 2009; Strasser et al. 2009). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has shown activation in regions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) associated with memory encoding in response to effective persuasive arguments (Fal et al. 2010b). Further, activation in the PFC in response to persuasive messages predicts behavior change (Falk et al. 2010a). Indeed, the accuracy of recall of anti-smoking PSAs correlates with PFC activation (Langleben et al. 2009), and increases in PFC activation while viewing anti-smoking PSAs predict quitting success (Chua et al. 2011). Therefore, factors influencing cognitive processing in the PFC might influence the effectiveness of anti-smoking PSAs.

Dopamine is one of the primary neurotransmitters in the PFC, and regulation of dopamine levels is a key component of optimizing PFC function (Cools & D’Esposito 2011). Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) degrades dopamine and is the primary regulator of dopamine levels in the PFC. A common polymorphism in the gene encoding COMT results in a valine to methionine substitution at codon 158 (Val158Met). The COMT Met allele is associated with reduced enzyme activity, leading to higher dopamine levels in the brain (Chen et al. 2004). COMT Met carriers outperform COMT Val homozygotes in tasks measuring working memory and PFC function (Egan et al. 2001; Goldberg et al. 2003). Furthermore, fMRI studies incorporating working memory tasks have shown greater increases in activation and less task-related deactivation in the PFC in COMT Val homozygotes at the same level of performance, a finding which has been interpreted as decreased neural efficiency in this group compared with COMT Met carriers (Bertolino et al. 2006; Pomarol-Clotet et al. 2010).

Another potential influence on PFC function is brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). BDNF is involved in synaptic transmission and plasticity and has been shown to influence hippocampal-dependent learning and memory (Savitz et al. 2006). The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism affects intracellular trafficking of pro-BDNF and secretion of the mature peptide (Egan et al. 2003); carriers of the BDNF Met allele show reduced hippocampal activity during memory encoding and retrieval tasks (Hariri et al. 2003; Hashimoto et al. 2008) and perform worse than BDNF Val homozygotes on a measure of PFC function (Rybakowski et al. 2003). Furthermore, Schofield et al. (2009) found that BDNF Met homozygotes showed delays in P300 latency in response to task-relevant target stimuli, which suggests an effect of this polymorphism on selective information processing.

On the basis of the demonstrated associations of COMT and BDNF with working memory and cognitive processing in the PFC, and the role of cognitive processing and PFC activity in the perceived effectiveness of PSAs, we investigated associations of COMT and BDNF genotype with self-reported message processing, perceived effectiveness and quitting intentions among smokers after viewing sets of PSAs. We hypothesized that we would see reduced processing and perceived effectiveness of PSAs and lower intentions to quit smoking among individuals carrying the COMT Val or BDNF Met alleles, which have been associated with decreased PFC function. We classified PSAs according to message sensation value (MSV) and argument strength (AS), two factors shown to influence individual responses to PSAs (Strasser et al. 2009), and examined interactions between genotype, MSV and AS on outcomes.

Methods

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References

Selection of anti-tobacco PSAs

PSA selection and study design were described previously (Strasser et al. 2009). Briefly, 569 cigarette smoking PSAs were acquired from several state and national health authorities. Three trained raters viewed each PSA for content and identified a subset of 99 PSAs that (1) promoted seeking smoking cessation treatment or portrayed the negative consequences of continuing to smoke, (2) targeted adults and (3) were 30 s in duration. These PSAs were rated for MSV features using a scoring template (visual range = 0–10, audio range = 0–5 and content range = 0–5) based on work by Morgan et al. (2003).

To classify PSAs by AS, trained raters viewed the PSAs to generate a single statement reflecting the central argument (or arguments) of each PSA (e.g. ‘If the health harms of smoking are not enough to get you to quit, consider quitting for your children and those you love’, and ‘Although you may think smoking helps you cope, if you don't soon quit you will eventually die’). Next, we conducted a shopping mall intercept survey of 300 current smokers to collect ratings of the transcribed central arguments from which an overall AS score was created for each PSA by taking the mean of the 36–38 individual scores recorded for each PSA (Zhao et al. 2011).

Four groups of PSAs were then created from the existing collection of 99 PSAs: (1) high MSV–high AS, (2) high MSV–low AS, (3) low MSV–high AS and (4) low MSV–low AS. PSAs exceeding 1 SD from the mean on each of the two dimensions were selected for use in the present study; 16 PSAs met this criterion (four in each group).

Participant selection

Smokers responding to recruitment flyers and advertisements participated in an initial telephone contact at which eligibility was determined. A total of 199 eligible individuals completed a single 90-min session. After giving informed consent, participants provided an exhaled breath carbon monoxide sample (Vitalograph, Lenexa, KS, USA) for biochemical verification of smoking status and a saliva sample for genotyping. Of these 199 participants, 122 self-identified as European American [EA, selected to reduce potential bias because of population stratification (Palmatier et al. 1999; Petryshen et al. 2010)]. Of these, genotype data (BDNF rs6265 and COMT rs4680) were collected from 120 participants. Genotypes were classified as Val/Val vs. */Met for both BDNF rs6265 and COMT rs4680 based on previous research showing significant cognitive differences between Val homozygotes and Met carriers for both genes (Colzato et al. 2010; Hariri et al. 2003; Loughead et al. 2009; Schofield et al. 2009).

Standard questionnaires (Lerman et al. 1997) were administered at the beginning of the session to assess demographics, smoking history and nicotine dependence (Fagerström test for nicotine dependence, which includes current smoking rate; Heatherton et al. 1991). Four PSAs were then presented through a 17-in computer monitor using MediaLab Research Software (Empirisoft, New York, NY, USA) with the participant seated in a comfortable chair approximately 1 m away. Each participant viewed one of the four sets of PSAs classified by MSV and AS. After viewing the PSAs, participants completed measures of cognitive processing, narrative processing and sensory processing of the PSAs (Andrews et al. 1990; Chaudhuri & Buck 1995; Palmgreen et al. 2002); affective response to the PSAs (Batra & Holbrook 1990; Chaudhuri & Buck 1995); perceived effectiveness of the PSA; recognition of content (Everett & Palmgreen 1995) and intentions to quit smoking (Fishbein et al. 2001; Norman et al. 1999; Yzer et al. 2003). Items measuring processing were accompanied by response scales ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much), and assessed thinking about the central message of the PSA (cognitive processing; five items, e.g. ‘Overall, how much did the PSA make you think about arguments for quitting smoking?’), the story told by the PSA (narrative processing; three items, e g., ‘Overall, how much did you pay attention to the characters in the PSA?’) and the sensory qualities of the PSA (sensory processing; three items, e g., ‘Overall, how much did you pay attention to the PSA's sound tracks?’). Perceived effectiveness was measured by four sets of seven items (one set for each PSA) accompanied by response scales running from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Intentions to quit were measured using two items accompanied by four-point response scales. Affective response was measured using four sets of 12 items (one set for each PSA) assessing emotional response to the PSA (e.g. ‘Did the PSA make you feel sad?’) accompanied by response scales ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely). For each construct described above (cognitive processing, narrative processing, sensory processing, perceived effectiveness, intentions to quit and affective response), participant responses were averaged across all items measuring that construct to generate a summary score, which was used for analysis. Recognition was assessed using five items; for purposes of analysis, a dichotomous recognition variable was used (answered all five items correctly vs. answered at least one item incorrectly).

Descriptive statistics were obtained for all variables. One-way anovas (analysis of variances) were used to test for differences across genotype groups in demographics, smoking history, quitting intentions, processing, perceived PSA effectiveness and recognition. Linear regression models of quitting intentions, perceived effectiveness, message processing as well as affective response and a logistic regression model of the dichotomized recognition measure were then performed. The predictors were age, nicotine dependence (continuous), education (college graduate = 1, non-college graduate = 0), MSV (low = 0, high = 1), AS (low = 0, high = 1) and the dichotomous versions of BDNF Val66Met (Val/Val = 0 and */Met = 1) and COMT Val158Met (*/Met = 0 and Val/Val = 1). The two-way interactions of the genotype variables with MSV and AS were also included. All predictors were entered as a block, after which nonsignificant (P > 0.05) interaction terms were allowed to drop out.

We investigated the possibility that the association of BDNF genotype with quitting intentions and perceived effectiveness was mediated by central processing using the method originally proposed by Baron and Kenny (1986) and applied to tobacco prevention research by MacKinnon et al. (2002). Using this method, mediation is shown when (1) the predictor is significantly associated with both the outcome and the mediator and (2) in a regression of outcome on predictor and mediator, the mediator is significantly associated with the outcome (which should reduce, or render nonsignificant, the effect of the predictor). To accomplish this, regression models of quitting intentions and perceived effectiveness were performed using BDNF genotype and central processing as predictors.

Results

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References

Demographics

In the final sample of 120 smokers, 69 (57.5%) were male and 40 (33.3%) graduated college. Mean age was 41.52 years (SD = 12.49), mean number of cigarettes smoked each day at baseline was 23.25 (SD = 16.06) and mean score at baseline on nicotine dependence was 5.13 (SD = 2.49).

Genotype distribution

The genotype distribution for BDNF Val66Met was 75 (62.5%) Val/Val, 40 (33.3%) Val/Met and 5 (4.2%) Met/Met. For COMT Val158Met the distribution was 29 (24.2%) Met/Met, 72 (60.0%) Val/Met and 19 (15.8%) Val/Val. Both genotypes were in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium. For hypothesis testing, both BDNF and COMT genotypes were dichotomized as Val/Val vs. */Met. Characteristics of demographics did not differ by BDNF genotype; however, there were differences in smoking rate and nicotine dependence by COMT genotype (Table 1). No differences were found in the distribution of genotypes across the study conditions.

Table 1.  Demographics by genotype
Variable BDNF Val66Met COMT Val158Met
Val/Val (n = 75)Val/Met (n = 40)Met/Met (n = 5)Val/Val (n = 19)Val/Met (n = 72)Met/Met (n = 29)
  1. * F for difference across these three means = 3.26, P = 0.042.

  2. F for difference across these three means = 3.74, P = 0.027.

Age, M (SD)40.96 (12.67)42.62 (12.60)41.00 (10.20)38.16 (12.65)43.43 (12.51)38.97 (11.82)
Female, N (%)32 (42.7)15 (37.5)4 (80.0)10 (52.6)26 (36.1)15 (51.7)
College graduate, N (%)25 (33.3)15 (37.5)0 (0.0)7 (36.8)23 (31.9)10 (34.5)
Cigarettes/day, M (SD)22.09 (14.51)25.95 (19.23)19.00 (7.42)16.05* (7.79)25.94* (19.32)21.28* (7.30)
Nicotine dependence, M (SD)5.03 (2.64)5.42 (2.23)4.40 (2.41)3.79 (1.99)5.26 (2.63)5.69 (2.19)

Genetic associations with self-report outcomes

In the multivariate models, BDNF genotype was associated significantly with quitting intentions (β = −0.31, t(112 df) = −3.38, P = 0.001), perceived effectiveness (β = −0.32, t(112 df) = −3.76, P < 0.001), central processing (β = −0.29, t(112 df) = −3.31, P = 0.001) and narrative processing (β = −0.19, t(111 df) = −2.02, P = 0.046); as shown in Fig. 1, the reduced-function BDNF Met allele was associated with lower scores on all of these measures. There were no significant main effects of COMT genotype; however, there was a significant interaction of COMT genotype by AS in the model of narrative processing (β = 0.27, t(111 df) = 2.02, P = 0.046), indicating that the COMT Val/Val genotype was associated with higher narrative processing scores in the high-AS condition, but not in the low-AS condition (where a trivial difference in the opposite direction was observed). Neither BDNF nor COMT genotype contributed to the models of sensory processing, affective response or recognition.

image

Figure 1. Association of outcome measures with genotype. Results are presented as mean scores ± SE. In the multivariate models, the dichotomous BDNF genotype (Val/Val vs. */Met) was significantly associated with quitting intentions (β = −0.31, P = 0.001), perceived effectiveness (β = −0.32, P < 0.001), central processing (β = −0.29, P = 0.001) and narrative processing (β = −0.19, P = 0.046).

Download figure to PowerPoint

As described earlier, to test the possibility that central processing might mediate the effect of BDNF genotype on perceived effectiveness and quitting intentions, additional regression models of perceived effectiveness and quitting intentions were performed using BDNF genotype and central processing as predictors. As shown in Table 2, in both of the models, the effect of central processing was highly significant, and the effect of BDNF genotype was reduced, becoming nonsignificant (β = −0.10, t(117 df) = −1.79, P = 0.076) in the model of perceived effectiveness.

Table 2.  Results of regression models testing mediation of the effect of BDNF Val66Met genotype on quitting intentions and perceived effectiveness by central processing
OutcomeEffect of BDNF Val66MetEffect of central processing
β t P β t P
Quitting intentions−0.18−2.120.0370.414.81<0.001
Perceived effectiveness−0.10−1.790.0760.8014.97<0.001

Discussion

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References

We report a significant association of BDNF Val66Met genotype with central processing, narrative processing and perceived effectiveness of the PSAs used in this study, and with quitting intentions; the reduced-function BDNF Met allele was associated with lower scores on all these measures. We have shown that central processing acts as a mediator of the effect of BDNF genotype on quitting intentions and perceived effectiveness; these results are independent upon nicotine dependence.

The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism affects intracellular trafficking and secretion of the mature BDNF peptide; the reduced-function BDNF Met allele impacts measures of memory encoding and retrieval, selective information processing, and general PFC function (Egan et al. 2003; Hariri et al. 2003; Rybakowski et al. 2003; Schofield et al. 2009). Delays in information processing and difficulties in memory encoding may negatively affect an individual's ability to follow and process the central message of a PSA, especially given that the short duration of the PSA requires this information to be processed quickly. It is possible that an individual who experiences more difficulty in processing the central argument may not find the PSA to be effective or persuasive, and thus the PSA would have little effect on quitting intentions.

The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism has also been shown to influence emotional response and emotional decision making (Gasic et al. 2009; Kang et al. 2010), and it is possible that the effects we have shown could be influenced by emotional processing of the PSAs used in the study. We did not see an association between BDNF genotype and affective response to the PSAs, and therefore we believe that the associations of this polymorphism with quitting intentions and perceived effectiveness in our study are primarily mediated by differences in cognitive processing between BDNF Val/Val homozygotes and Met carriers. However, future studies which more closely examine the influence of BDNF Val66Met on emotional response to PSAs may be of interest.

While we did not see a main effect of COMT Val158Met genotype on the primary outcomes of the study, we did note a significant interaction of COMT genotype by AS in the model of narrative processing; individuals homozygous for the COMT Val allele reported higher narrative processing in the high-AS condition, but not in the low-AS condition. The COMT Val allele encodes the normally functioning protein; dopamine levels in the PFC of COMT Val homozygotes are lower than in COMT Met allele carriers. Most studies of the COMT Val158Met polymorphism have focused on deficits in working memory in COMT Val homozygotes (Egan et al. 2001; Goldberg et al. 2003). However, a recent study suggests that the rapid regulation of dopamine levels in the COMT Val homozygotes may afford greater cognitive flexibility; COMT Val/Val individuals were faster and more efficient than COMT Met carriers in a task-switching paradigm (Colzato et al. 2010). It is therefore possible that COMT Val homozygotes in our study were able to more rapidly shift cognitive resources toward processing information in a PSA if the argument was strong enough to warrant consideration.

Generally, the effects of AS on the outcomes in our models were not as great as we expected. This could be due to the fact that the PSAs selected for this study were actual PSAs designed for, and used in, anti-smoking campaigns. Because no anti-smoking campaign would intentionally use extremely weak arguments as an intervention, the observed variation in AS across PSAs (while validated; Zhao et al. 2011) was relatively limited.

A limitation of this study is that we rely on accurate self-assessment of cognitive processing; our measures are not direct or objective measurements of cognitive processing. Despite this, we have no reason to suspect differences in self-report based on genotype, so the likelihood of bias is low, and the measures used here have shown good construct validity in previous research (Andrews et al. 1990; Chaudhuri & Buck 1995; Palmgreen et al. 2002). Another limitation is that time since last cigarette was not controlled. Nicotine administration is known to improve attention (Heishman et al. 2010) and nicotine withdrawal syndrome includes cognitive deficits (Leventhal et al. 2010); inconsistent levels of nicotine withdrawal among our participants may have added to error variance. However, in real-life situations smokers may view PSAs at any time; therefore, our findings may more closely reflect a real-world effect. Finally, our sample consisted entirely of non-treatment seeking smokers viewing ads with a smoking cessation theme. Chronic nicotine exposure has been shown to alter levels of BDNF (Bhang et al. 2010; Czubak et al. 2009), which may alter the degree to which BDNF genotype influences cognitive processing. Furthermore, some anti-smoking PSAs employ different themes, such as smoking prevention or emphasizing the dangers of second-hand smoke, and are directed toward other groups (adolescents who have not started smoking or non-smokers). Therefore, our results may not apply to all anti-smoking PSAs or target audiences.

In conclusion, we have shown a significant association of BDNF Val66Met genotype with self-report outcomes of cognitive processing independent upon nicotine dependence, and a significant interaction of COMT Val158Met genotype by AS on narrative processing. Our findings are particularly interesting in light of recent studies suggesting an association between BDNF and COMT genotypes and smoking. The BDNF Met allele has been associated with increased risk of smoking initiation and nicotine dependence (Beuten et al. 2005; Lang et al. 2007; Novak et al. 2010), although not in all populations (Montag et al. 2008); the COMT Val allele has been associated with smoking status (Nedic et al. 2010) and decreased quit rates in smoking cessation studies (Colilla et al. 2005), possibly mediated by an increased susceptibility to cognitive symptoms following nicotine abstinence (Loughead et al. 2009). Given these associations, further consideration of genetic influences on cognitive processing of anti-smoking PSAs may be useful to increase PSA effectiveness for the target population. Moreover, our research may have broader implications concerning health messaging in at-risk populations. Future research into genotype associations with response to other health-behavior messages (e.g. healthy eating for obese populations) may provide valuable insights for public health campaigns.

References

  1. Top of page
  2. Abstract
  3. Methods
  4. Results
  5. Discussion
  6. References
  • Andrews, J.C., Durvasula, S. & Akhter, S.H. (1990) A framework for conceptualizing and measuring the involvement construct in advertising research. J Advertising 19, 2740.
  • Baron, R.M. & Kenny, D.A. (1986) The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51, 11731182.
  • Batra, R. & Holbrook, M.B. (1990) Developing a typology of affective response to advertising. Psychol Market 7, 1125.
  • Beuten, J., Ma, J.Z., Payne, T.J., Dupont, R.T., Quezada, P., Huang, W., Crews, K.M. & Li, M.D. (2005) Significant association of BDNF haplotypes in European-American male smokers but not in European-American female or African-American smokers. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 139B, 7380.
  • Bhang, S.Y., Choi, S.W. & Ahn, J.H. (2010) Changes in plasma brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels in smokers after smoking cessation. Neurosci Lett 468, 711.
  • Bertolino, A., Blasi, G., Latorre, V., Rubino, V., Rampino, A., Sinibaldi, L., Caforio, G., Petruzzella, V., Pizzuti, A., Scarabino, T., Nardini, M., Weinberger, D.R. & Dallapiccola, B. (2006) Additive effects of genetic variation in dopamine regulating genes on working memory cortical activity in human brain. J Neurosci 26, 39183922.
  • Biener, L., Harris, J.E. & Hamilton, W. (2000) Impact of the Massachusetts tobacco control programme: population based trend analysis. BMJ 321, 351354.
  • Chaudhuri, A. & Buck, R. (1995) Media differences in rational and emotional responses to advertising. J Broadcast Electron 39, 109125.
  • Chen, J., Lipska, B.K., Halim, N., Ma, Q.D., Matsumoto, M., Melhem, S., Kolachana, B.S., Hyde, T.M., Herman, M.M., Apud, J., Egan, M.F., Kleinman, J.E. & Weinberger, D.R. (2004) Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am J Hum Genet 75, 807821.
  • Chua, H.F., Ho, S.S., Jasinska, A.J., Polk, T.A., Welsh, R.C., Liberzon, I. & Stretcher, V.J. (2011) Self-related neural response to tailored smoking-cessation messages predicts quitting. Nat Neurosci 14, 426427.
  • Colilla, S., Lerman, C., Shields, P.G., Jepson, C., Rukstalis, M., Berlin, J., DeMichele, A., Bunin, G., Strom, B.L. & Rebbeck, T.R. (2005) Association of catechol-O-methyltransferase with smoking cessation in two independent studies of women. Pharmacogenet Genomics 15, 393398.
  • Colzato, L.S., Waszak, F., Nieuwenhuis, S., Posthuma, D. & Hommel, B. (2010) The flexible mind is associated with the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val158Met polymorphism: evidence for a role of dopamine in the control of task-switching. Neuropsychologia 48, 27642768.
  • Cools, R. & D’Esposito, M. (2011) Inverted U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol Psychiatry 69, e113e125.
  • Czubak, A., Nowakowska, E., Kus, K., Burda, K., Metelska, J., Baer-Dubowska, W. & Cichocki, M. (2009) Influences of chronic venlafaxine, olanzapine and nicotine on the hippocampal and cortical concentrations of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). Pharmacol Rep 61, 10171023.
  • Egan, M.F., Goldberg, T.E., Kolachana, B.S., Callicott, J.H., Mazzanti, C.M., Straub, R.E., Goldman, D. & Weinberger, D.R. (2001) Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98, 69176922.
  • Egan, M.F., Kojima, M., Callicott, J.H., Goldberg, T.E., Kolachana, B.S., Bertolino, A., Zaitsev, E., Gold, B., Goldman, D., Dean, M., Lu, B. & Weinberger, D.R. (2003) The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell 112, 257269.
  • Everett, M. & Palmgreen, P. (1995) Influences of sensation seeking, message sensation value, and program context on effectiveness of anti-cocaine public service announcements. Health Commun 7, 225248.
  • Falk, E.B., Berkman, E.T., Mann, T., Harrison, B. & Lieberman, M.D. (2010a) Predicting persuasion-induced behavior change from the brain. J Neurosci 30, 84218424.
  • Falk, E.B., Rameson, L., Berkman, E.T., Liao, B., Kang, Y., Inagaki, T.K. & Lieberman, M.D. (2010b) The neural correlates of persuasion: a common network across cultures and media. J Cogn Neurosci 22, 24472459.
  • Fishbein, M., Hennessy, M., Kamb, M., Bolan, G.A., Hoxworth, T., Iatesta, M., Rhodes, F., Zenilman, J.M. & Project Respect Study Group. (2001) Using intervention theory to model factors influencing behavior change. Project RESPECT. Eval Health Prof 24, 363384.
  • Gasic, G.P., Smoller, J.W., Perlis, R.H., Sun, M., Lee, S., Kim, B.W., Lee, M.J., Holt, D.J., Blood, A.J., Makris, N., Kennedy, D.K., Hoge, R.D., Calhoun, J., Fava, M., Gusella, J.F. & Breiter, H.C. (2009) BDNF, relative preference, and reward circuitry responses to emotional communication. Am J Med Genet Part B 150B, 762781.
  • Goldberg, T.E., Egan, M.F., Gscheidle, T., Coppola, R., Weickert, T., Kolachana, B.S., Goldman, D. & Weinberger, D.R. (2003) Executive subprocesses in working memory: relationship to catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psych 60, 889896.
  • Hariri, A.R., Goldberg, T.E., Mattay, V.S., Kolachana, B.S., Callicott, J.H., Egan, M.F. & Weinberger, D.R. (2003) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism affects human memory-related hippocampal activity and predicts memory performance. J Neurosci 23, 66906694.
  • Hashimoto, R., Moriguchi, Y., Yamashita, F., Mori, T., Nemoto, K., Okada, T., Hori, H., Noguchi, H., Kunugi, H. & Ohnishi, T. (2008) Dose-dependent effect of the Val66Met polymorphism of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene on memory-related hippocampal activity. Neurosci Res 61, 360367.
  • Heatherton, T.F., Kozlowski, L.T., Frecker, R.C. & Fagerstrom, K.O. (1991) The Fagerstrom test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom tolerance questionnaire. Br J Addict 86, 11191127.
  • Heishman, S.J., Kleykamp, B.A. & Singleton, E.G. (2010) Meta-analysis of the acute effects of nicotine and smoking on human performance. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 210, 453469.
  • Kang, J.I., Namkoong, K., Ha, R.Y., Jhung, K., Kim, Y.T. & Kim, S.J. (2010) Influence of BDNF and COMT polymorphisms on emotional decision making. Neuropharmacology 58, 11091113.
  • Lang, U.E., Sander, T., Lohoff, F.W., Hellweg, R., Bajbouj, M., Winterer, G. & Gallinat, J. (2007) Association of the met66 allele of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) with smoking. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 190, 433439.
  • Langleben, D.D., Loughead, J.W., Ruparel, K., Hakun, J.G., Busch-Winokour, S., Holloway, M.B., Strasser, A.A., Cappella, J.N. & Lerman, C. (2009) Reduced prefrontal and temporal processing and recall of high “sensation value” ads. NeuroImage 46, 219225.
  • Lerman, C., Gold, K., Audrain, J., Lin, T.H., Boyd, N.R., Orleans, C.T., Wilfond, B., Louben, G. & Caporaso, N. (1997) Incorporating biomarkers of exposure and genetic susceptibility into smoking cessation treatment: effects on smoking-related cognitions, emotions, and behavior change. Health Psychol 16, 8799. 1.
  • Leshner, G. & Cheng, I.H. (2009) The effects of frame, appeal, and outcome extremity of antismoking messages on cognitive processing. Health Commun 24, 219227. 3.
  • Leventhal, A.M., Waters, A.J., Moolchan, E.T., Heishman, S.J. & Pickworth, W.B. (2010) A quantitative analysis of subjective, cognitive, and physiological manifestations of the acute tobacco abstinence syndrome. Addict Behav 35, 11201130.
  • Loughead, J., Wileyto, E.P., Valdez, J.N., Sanborn, P., Tang, K., Strasser, A.A., Ruparel, K., Ray, R., Gur, R.C. & Lerman, C. (2009) Effect of abstinence challenge on brain function and cognition in smokers differs by COMT genotype. Mol Psychiatry 14, 820826.
  • MacKinnon, D.P., Taborga, M.P. & Morgan-Lopez, A.A. (2002) Mediation designs for tobacco prevention research. Drug Alcohol Depend 68, S69S83.
  • Messer, K., Pierce, J.P., Zhu, S.H., Hartman, A.M., Al-Delaimy, W.K., Trinidad, D.R. & Gilpin, E.A. (2007) The California Tobacco Control Program's effect on adult smokers: (1) smoking cessation. Tob Control 16, 8590.
  • Montag, C., Basten, U., Stelzel, C., Fiebach, C.J. & Reuter, M. (2008) The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and smoking. Neurosci Lett 442, 3033.
  • Morgan, S.E., Palmgreen, P., Stephenson, M.T., Lorch, E.P. & Huyle, R.H. (2003) The relationship between message sensation value and perceived message sensation value: the effect of formal message features on subjective evaluations of anti-drug public service announcements. J Commun 53, 512526.
  • Nedic, G., Nikolac, M., Borovecki, F., Hajnsek, S., Muck-Seler, D. & Pivac, N. (2010) Association study of a functional catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism and smoking in healthy Caucasian subjects. Neurosci Lett 473, 216219.
  • Novak, G., LeBlanc, M., Zai, C., Shaikh, S., Renou, J., DeLuca, V., Bulgin, N., Kennedy, J.L. & Le Foll, B. (2010) Association of polymorphisms in the BDNF, DRD1 and DRD3 genes with tobacco smoking in schizophrenia. Ann Hum Genet 74, 291298.
  • Norman, P., Conner, M. & Bell, R. (1999) The theory of planned behavior and smoking cessation. Health Psychol 18, 8994.
  • Palmatier, M.A., Kang, A.M. & Kidd, K.K. (1999) Global variation in the frequencies of functionally different catechol-O-methyltransferase alleles. Biol Psychiatry 46, 557567.
  • Palmgreen, P., Stephenson, M.T., Everett, M.W., Baseheart, J.R. & Francies, R. (2002) Perceived message sensation value (PMSV) and the dimensions and validation of a PMSV scale. Health Commun 1 4, 403428.
  • Petryshen, T.L., Sabeti, P.C., Aldinger, K.A., Fry, B., Fan, J.B., Schaffner, S.F., Waggoner, S.G., Tahl, A.R. & Sklar, P. (2010) Population genetic study of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene. Mol Psychiatry 15, 810815.
  • Pomarol-Clotet, E., Fatjó-Vilas, M., McKenna, P.J., Monté, G.C., Sarró, S., Ortiz-Gil, J., Aguirre, C., Gomar, J.J., Guerrero, A., Landin, R., Capdevila, A., Fañanás, L. & Salvador, R. (2010) COMT Val158Met polymorphism in relation to activation and de-activation in the prefrontal cortex: a study in patients with schizophrenia and healthy subjects. NeuroImage 53, 899907.
  • Rybakowski, J.K., Borkowska, A., Czerski, P.M., Skibinska, M. & Hauser, J. (2003) Polymorphism of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene and performance on a cognitive prefrontal test in bipolar patients. Bipolar Disord 5, 468472.
  • Savitz, J., Solms, M. & Ramesar, R. (2006) The molecular genetics of cognition: dopamine, COMT and BDNF. Genes Brain Behav 5, 311328.
  • Schofield, P.R., Williams, L.M., Paul, R.H., Gatt, J.M., Brown, K., Luty, A., Cooper, N., Grieve, S., Dobson-Stone, C., Morris, C., Kuan, S.A. & Gordon, E. (2009) Disturbances in selective information processing associated with the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism: evidence from cognition, the P300 and fronto-hippocampal systems. Biol Psychol 80, 176188.
  • Siegel, M. (1998) Mass media antismoking campaigns: a powerful tool for health promotion. Ann Intern Med 129, 128132.
  • Strasser, A.A., Cappella, J.N., Jepson, C., Fishbein, M., Tang, K.Z., Han, E. & Lerman, C. (2009) Experimental evaluation of antitobacco PSAs: effects of message content and format on physiological and behavioral outcomes. Nicotine Tob Res 11, 293302.
  • World Health Organization. (2009) WHO report on the global tobacco epidemic: Implementing smoke free environments. Geneva, World Health Organization.
  • Yzer, M.C., Cappella, J.N., Fishbein, M., Hornik, R. & Ahern, R.K. (2003) The effectiveness of gateway communications in anti-marijuana campaigns. J Health Commun 8, 129143.
  • Zhao, X., Strasser, A.A., Cappella, J.N., Lerman, C. & Fishbein, M. (2011) A measure of perceived argument strength: reliability and validity in health communication contexts. Commun Methods Measures 5, 4875.