SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Amazeen, E. L., Schmidt, R. C., & Turvey, M. T. (1995). Frequency detuning of the phase entrainment dynamics of visually coupled rhythmic movements. Biological Cybernetics, 72, 511518.
  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
  • Asch, S. (1952). Social psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall.
  • Axelrod, R. (1984). Evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books.
  • Axelrod, R., & Dion, D. (1988). The further evolution of cooperation. Science, 242(4884), 13851390.
  • Baron, R. M. (2007). Situating coordination and cooperation between ecological and social psychology. Ecological Psychology, 19(2), 179199.
  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(2), 113127.
  • Bekkering, H., De Bruijn, E., Cuipers, R., Newman-Norlund, R., Van Schie, H., & Muelenbroek, R. (2009). Joint action: Neurocognitive mechanisms supporting human interaction. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 340352.
  • Benet-Martinez, V., & John, O. P. (1998). Los Cinco Grandes across cultures and ethnic groups: Multitrait multimethod analyses of the Big Five in Spanish and English. Journal of personality and social psychology, 75(3), 729750.
  • Bernieri, F. J. (1988). Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher-student interactions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12(2), 120138.
  • Bernieri, F. J., Gillis, J. S., Davis, J. M., & Grahe, J. E. (1996). Dyad rapport and the accuracy of its judgment across situations: A lens model analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(1), 110129.
  • Bingham, G. P. (2004). A perceptually driven dynamical model of bimanual rhythmic movements (and phase entrainment). Ecological Psychology, 16, 4553.
  • Brennan, S. E., & Hanna, J. E. (2009). Partner-specific adaptation in dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 274291.
  • Brooks, R. (1991). Intelligence without representation. Artificial Intelligence, 47, 139159.
  • Carpenter, M. (2009). Just how joint is joint action in infancy? Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 380391.
  • Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2001). Understanding of others’ intentions in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 589599.
  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893910.
  • Chartrand, T. L., & Jefferis, V. E. (2003). Consequences of automatic goal pursuit and the case of nonconscious mimicry. In J. P.Forgas (Ed.), Social judgments: Implicit and explicit processes (pp. 290305). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Clark, A. (1998). Embodied, situated, and distributed cognition. In W.Bechtel & G.Graham (Eds.), A companion to cognitive science (pp. 506517). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
  • Van Dijk, J., Kerkhofs, R., Van Rooij, I., & Haselager, P. (2008). Can there be such a thing as embodied embedded cognitive neuroscience? Theory & Psychology, 18(3), 297.
  • Fajen, B. R., Riley, M. A., & Turvey, M. T. (in press). Information, affordances, and the control of action in sport. International Journal of Sports Psychology.
  • Fowler, C. A., Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Shockley, K. (2008). Language use, coordination, and the emergence of cooperative action. In A.Fuchs & V. K.Jirsa (Eds.), Coordination: Neural, behavioral and social dynamics (pp. 261279). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
  • Galantucci, B. (2009). Experimental semiotics: A new approach for studying communication as a form of joint action. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 393410.
  • Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 396.
  • Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2009). Joint action, interactive alignment, and dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 292304.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology (pp. 6782). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
  • Glenberg, A. M. (2008). Toward the integration of bodily states, language, and action. In G. R.Semin & E. R.Smith (Eds.), Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 4370). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Good, J. M. M. (2007). The affordances for social psychology of the ecological approach to social psychology. Theory & Psychology, 17, 265295.
  • Grossberg, S., & Seidman, D. (2006). Neural dynamics of autistic behaviors: Cognitive, emotional, and timing substrates. Psychological Review, 113, 483525.
  • Haken, H., Kelso, J. A. S., & Bunz, H. (1985). A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics, 51, 347356.
  • Heft, H. (2007). The social constitution of perceiver-environment reciprocity. Ecological Psychology, 19, 85105.
  • Von Holst, E. (1939/1973). Relative coordination as a phenomenon and as a method of analysis of central nervous system function. In R. Martin (Ed.), The collected papers of Eric von Holst (Vol.1, pp. 33135). Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami Press.
  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Issartel, J., Marin, L., Bardainne, T., Gaillot, P., & Cadopi, M. (2005a). A new method for studying non-stationary signals in human movement: The cross-wavelet transform. In H. Heft & K. L. Marsh (Eds.), Studies in perception and action VIII (pp. 207210). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Issartel, J., Marin, L., & Cadopi, M. (2005b, July). Interpersonal coordination in a specific task: A dance improvisation. Paper presented at the 13th International Conference on Perception and Action, Monterey, California.
  • Ivry, R. B., & Richardson, R. E. (2002). Temporal control and coordination: The multiple timer model. Brain and Cognition, 48, 117132.
  • Kean, K. J. (2000). An investigation into the interpersonal kinematics of cooperation and competition. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 60(9-B), 4965.
  • Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns. Cambridge, MA.: MIT Press.
  • Lopresti-Goodman, S., Richardson, M. J., Baron, R. M., Carello, C., & Marsh, K. L. (2009). Task constraints on affordance boundaries. Motor Control, 13, 117.
  • Lopresti-Goodman, S., Richardson, M. J., Silva, P. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2008). Period basin of entrainment for unintentional visual coordination. Journal of Motor Behavior, 30, 310.
  • Macrae, C. N., Duffy, O. K., Miles, L. K., & Lawrence, J. (2008). A case of hand waving: Action synchrony and person perception. Cognition, 109, 152156.
  • Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., Baron, R. M., & Schmidt, R. C. (2006a). Contrasting approaches to perceiving and acting with others. Ecological Psychology, 18, 137.
  • Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., & Schmidt, R. C. (2006b, October). Emergence of sociality from the dynamics of bodily synchrony. In E. Smith (Chair), Social embodiment. Invited symposium presented at the meeting of the Society of Experimental Social Psychology, Philadelphia, PA.
  • Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007, April). Social connection through joint action and interpersonal coordination. Paper presented at the 2nd Joint Action Meeting, Newark, NJ: Rutgers University.
  • Meschner, F., Kerzel, D., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination. Nature, 414, 6972.
  • Michaels, C. F., & Carello, C. (1981). Direct perception. Enqlewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • Newtson, D., Hairfield, J., Bloomingdale, J., & Cutino, S. (1987). The structure of action and interaction. Social Cognition, 5(3), 191237.
  • Ravizza, S. (2003). Movement and lexical access: Do noniconic gestures aid in retrieval? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 610615.
  • Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Baron, R. M. (2007a). Judging and actualizing intrapersonal and interpersonal affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 33, 845859.
  • Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007b). Rocking together: Dynamics of unintentional and intentional interpersonal coordination. Human Movement Science, 26, 867891.
  • Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2005). Effects of visual and verbal couplings on unintentional interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 31, 6279.
  • Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (in press). Challenging the egocentric view of coordinated perceiving, acting, and knowing. In L. F.Barrett, B.Mesquita, & E.Smith (Eds.), The mind in context. New York: GuilFord Press.
  • Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2003). When prejudice does not pay: Effects of interracial contact on executive function. Psychological Science, 14(3), 287290.
    Direct Link:
  • Richardson, M. J., Shockley, K., Fajen, B. R., Riley, M. A., & Turvey, M. T. (2008). Ecological psychology: Six principles for an embodied-embedded approach to behavior. In P.Calvo & T.Gomila (Eds.), Elsevier hand book of new directions in cognitive science. New York: Elsevier.
  • Riggio, R. E. (1996). Assessment of basic social skills. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 549660.
  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169192.
  • Roerdink, M., Peper, C. E., & Beek, P. J. (2005). Effects of correct and transformed visual feedback on rhythmic visuo-motor tracking: Tracking performance and visual search behavior. Human Movement Science, 24, 379402.
  • Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137162.
  • Schmidt, R. C. (1989). Dynamical constraints on the coordination of rhythmic limb movements between two people. Doctoral dissertation, University of Connecticut, 1988. Dissertation Abstracts International, 50, 2128.
  • Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Scaffolds for social meaning. Ecological Psychology, 19, 137151.
  • Schmidt, R. C., Bienvenu, M., Fitzpatrick, P. A., & Amazeen, P. G. (1998). A comparison of intra- and interpersonal interlimb coordination: Coordination breakdowns and coupling strength. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24(3), 884900.
  • Schmidt, R. C., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1990). Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 16(2), 227247.
  • Schmidt, R. C., Christianson, N., Carello, C., & Baron, R. (1994). Effects of social and physical variables on between-person visual coordination. Ecological Psychology, 6(3), 159183.
  • Schmidt, R. C., & O’ Brien, B. (1997). Evaluating the dynamics of unintended interpersonal coordination. Ecological Psychology, 9(3), 189206.
  • Schmidt, R. C., & Richardson, M. J. (2008). Dynamics of interpersonal coordination. In A.Fuchs & V. K.Jirsa (Eds.), Coordination: Neural, behavioral and social dynamics (pp. 281307). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
  • Schmidt, R. C., Richardson, M. J., Arsenault, C. A., & Galantucci, B. (2007). Visual tracking and entrainment to an environmental rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 33, 860870.
  • Schmidt, R. C., & Turvey, M. T. (1994). Phase-entrainment dynamics of visually coupled rhythmic movements. Biological cybernetics, 70(4), 369376.
  • Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2009). Prediction in joint action: What, when, and where. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 353367.
  • Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., Stumpf, L., & Prinz, W. (2005). Far from action-blind: Representation of others’ actions in individuals with autism. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 122.
  • Semin, G. R. (2007). Grounding communication: Synchrony. In A. W.Kruglanski & E. T.Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 630644). New York: Guilford.
  • Semin, G. R., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Grounding social cognition: Synchronization, coordination, and co-regulation. In G. R.Semin & E. R.Smith (Eds.), Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 119147). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Semin, G. R., & Smith, E. R. (2008). Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Semjen, A., & Ivry, R. B. (2001). The coupled oscillator model of between-hand coordination in alternate hand-tapping: A reappraisal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 27, 251265.
  • Shintel, H., & Keysar, B. (2009). Less is more: A minimalist account of joint action in communication. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 260273.
  • Shockley, K., Baker, A. A., Richardson, M. J., & Fowler, C. A. (2007). Articulatory constraints on interpersonal postural coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 33, 201208.
  • Shockley, K., Santana, M. V., & Fowler, C. A. (2003). Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(2), 326332.
  • Strogatz, S. H. (1994). Nonlinear dynamics and chaos. Cambridge, MA: Perseus.
  • Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of groups. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Tognoli, E., Lagarde, J., DeGuzman, C., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2007). The phi complex as a neuromarker of human social coordination. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 81908195.
  • Turvey, M. T. (1990). Coordination. American Psychologist, 45(8), 938953.
  • Turvey, M. T. (2005). Theory of brain and behavior in the 21st century: No ghost, no machine. Japanese Journal of Ecological Psychology, 2, 6979.
  • Turvey, M. T., & Carello, C. (1986). The ecological approach to perceiving-acting: A pictorial essay. Acta Psychologica, 63(1, 3), 133155.
  • Turvey, M. T., & Fonseca, S. (in press). Nature of motor control: Perspectives and issues. In D.Sternad (Ed.), Progress in motor control: A multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 93126). New York: Springer-Verlag.
  • Van Ulzen, N. R., Lamoth, C. J. C., Daffertshofer, A., Semin, G. R., & Beek, P. J. (2008). Characteristics of instructed and uninstructed interpersonal coordination while walking side-by-side. Neuroscience Letters, 432(2), 8893.
  • Warner, R. M. (1996). Coordinated cycles in behavior and physiology during face-to-face social interactions. In J. H.Watt & C. A.VanLear (Eds.), Dynamic patterns in communication processes (pp. 327352). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Warren, W. H. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 683703.
  • Warren, W. H. (2006). The dynamics of perception and action. Psychological Review, 113, 358389.
  • Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2000). CyberOstracism: The effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 748762.
  • Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., & Singh, T. (2004). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 285299.
  • Wimmers, R. H., Beek, P. J., & Van Wieringen, P. C. (1992). Phase transitions in rhythmic tracking movements: A case of unilateral coupling. Human Movement Science, 11, 217226.
  • Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149(Whole No. 3681), 269274.