SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Andersen, R. A., Essick, G. K., & Siegel, R. M. (1985). The encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. Science, 230, 456458.
  • Andersen, R. A., & Mountcastle, V. B. (1983). The influence of the angle of gaze upon the excitability of the light-sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 3, 532548.
  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Anderson, J. R. (2007). How can the mind exist in a physical universe? New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Attneave, F., & Curlee, T. E. (1983). Locational representation in imagery: A moving spot task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 2030.
  • Baciu, M., Koenig, O., Vernier, M. P., Bedoin, N., Rubin, C., & Segebarth, C. (1999). Categorical and coordinate spatial relations: FMRI evidence for hemispheric specialization. Neuroreport, 10, 13731378.
  • Ball, J., Myers, C. W., Heiberg, A., Cooke, N. J., Matessa, M., & Freiman, M. (2009). The synthetic teammate project. Proceedings of the 18th annual conference on behavior representation in modeling and simulation. Sundance, UT: BRIMS.
  • Barkowsky, T., & Freksa, C. (1997). Cognitive requirements on making and interpreting maps. In S. Hirtle & A. Frank (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (pp. 347361). Berlin: Springer.
  • Bejar, I. I. (1990). A generative analysis of a three-dimensional spatial task. Applied Psychological Measurement, 14, 237245.
  • Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 128.
  • Brooks, L. R. (1968). Spatial and verbal components in the act of recall. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 22, 349368.
  • Burgess, N. (2006). Spatial memory: How egocentric and allocentric combine. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(12), 551557.
  • Burgess, N., Donnett, J. G., Jeffery, K. J., & O’Keefe, J. (1999). Robotic and neuronal simulation of the hippocampus and rat navigation. In N. Burgess & K. J. Jeffery (Eds.), The hippocampal and parietal foundations of spatial cognition (pp. 149166). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Daprati, E., & Gentilucci, M. (1997). Grasping an illusion. Neuropsychologia, 35, 15771582.
  • Dimperio, E., Gunzelmann, G., & Harris, J. (2008). An initial evaluation of a cognitive model of UAV reconnaissance. In J. Hansberger (Ed.), Proceedings of the 17th conference on behavior representation in modeling and simulation (pp. 165173). Orlando, FL: Simulation Interoperability Standards Organization.
  • Diwadkar, V. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (2000). Collaborative activity between parietal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in dynamic spatial working memory revealed by fMRI. NeuroImage, 12, 8599.
  • Fincham, J. M., Carter, C. S., van Veen, V., Stenger, V. A., & Anderson, J. R. (2002). Neural mechanisms of planning: A computational analysis using event-related fMRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(5), 33463351.
  • Franklin, N., & Tversky, B. (1990). Searching imagined environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119(1), 6376.
  • Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006). Numbers and space: A computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 3244.
  • Glasgow, J. I., & Papadias, D. (1992). Computational imagery. Cognitive Science, 17(3), 355394.
  • Gunzelmann, G. (2008). Strategy generalization across orientation tasks: Testing a computational cognitive model. Cognitive Science, 32(5), 835861.
  • Gunzelmann, G., & Anderson, J. R. (2006). Location matters: Why target location impacts performance in orientation tasks. Memory & Cognition, 34(1), 4159.
  • Gunzelmann, G., & Lyon, D. R. (2006). Qualitative and quantitative reasoning and instance-based learning in spatial orientation. In R. Sun & N. Miyake (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 303308). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Gunzelmann, G., & Lyon, D. R. (2007). Mechanisms of human spatial competence. In T. Barkowsky, M. Knauff, G. Ligozat, & D. Montello (Eds.), Spatial cognition V: Reasoning, action, interaction. Lecture notes in Artificial Intelligence #4387 (pp. 288307). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
  • Gunzelmann, G., & Mohebbi, R. (2010). Spatial encoding in briefly presented schematic displays [Abstract]. In Poster book: Association for Psychological Science 22nd annual convention. (pp. 179). Washington, DC: Association for Psychological Science.
  • Harrison, A. M., & Schunn, C. D. (2003). ACT-R/S: Look Ma, no “cognitive map”! In F. Detje, D. Doerner, & H. Schaub (Eds.), Proceedings of the fifth international conference on cognitive modeling (pp. 129134). Bamberg, Germany: Universitats-Verlag Bamberg.
  • Hartley, T., Trinkler, I., & Burgess, N. (2004). Geometric determinants of human spatial memory. Cognition, 94(1), 3975.
  • Hegarty, M. (1992). Mental animation: Inferring motion from static diagrams of mechanical systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18(5), 10841102.
  • Huttenlocher, J., & Presson, C. C. (1979). The coding and transformation of spatial information. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 375394.
  • Kerr, N. H. (1987). Locational representation in imagery: The third dimension. Memory and Cognition, 15, 521530.
  • Kerr, N. H. (1993). Rate of imagery processing in two versus three dimensions. Memory and Cognition, 21, 467476.
  • Klatzky, R. L. (1998). Allocentric and egocentric spatial representations: Definitions, distinctions, and interconnections. In C. Freksa, C. Habel & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition – An interdisciplinary approach to representation and processing of spatial knowledge (Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence 1404) (pp. 117). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
  • Klatzky, R. L., Wu, B., & Stetten, G. (2008). Spatial representation from perception and cognitive mediation: The case of ultrasound. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 359364.
    Direct Link:
  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Kosslyn, S. M., Sokolov, M. A., & Chen, J. C. (1989). The lateralization of BRIAN: A computational theory and model of visual hemispheric specialization. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing comes of age: The impact of Herbert Simon (pp. 329). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., & Ganis, G. (2006). The case for mental imagery. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  • Levine, M., Jankovic, I. N., & Palij, M. (1982). Principles of spatial problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 157175.
  • Levine, M., Marchom, I., & Hanley, G. L. (1984). The placement and misplacement of you-are-here maps. Environment & Behavior, 16, 139157.
  • Lyon, D. (2009). Crowding in the mental image: Evidence for a vision-like representation? [Abstract]. In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st annual conference of the cognitive science society (p. 951). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Lyon, D. R., & Gunzelmann, G. (2009). Visualizing egocentric paths: A computational model. In A. Howes, D. Peebles, & R. Cooper (Eds.), Proceedings of the ninth international conference on cognitive modeling. Manchester, UK: University of Manchester.
  • Lyon, D. R., Gunzelmann, G., & Gluck, K. A. (2006). Virtual travel does not enhance spatial working memory for landmark-free paths. In R. Sun & N. Miyake (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 2550). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Lyon, D. R., Gunzelmann, G., & Gluck, K. A. (2007). Visualizing egocentric vs. exocentric path descriptions. In D. S. McNamara & G. Trafton (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (p. 1811). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Lyon, D. R., Gunzelmann, G., & Gluck, K. A. (2008). A computational model of spatial visualization capacity. Cognitive Psychology, 57, 122152.
  • Malinowski, J. C., & Gillespie, W. T. (2001). Individual differences in performance on a large-scale, real-world wayfinding task. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21, 7382.
  • Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby: II. Conceptual Primitives. Psychological Review, 99(4), 587604.
  • Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1993). Visual pathways to perception and action. Progress in Brain Research, 95, 317337.
  • Mou, W., & McNamara, T. (2002). Intrinsic frames of reference in spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 162170.
  • Mulliken, G. H., Musallam, S., & Andersen, R. A. (2008). Decoding trajectories from posterior parietal cortex ensembles. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(48), 1291312926.
  • Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • O’Keefe, J., & Dostrovsky, J. (1971). The hippocampus as a spatial map: Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely moving rat. Brain Research, 34, 171174.
  • O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Péruch, P., & Lapin, E. A. (1993). Route knowledge in different spatial frames of reference. Acta Psychologica, 84, 253269.
  • Rodríguez, F., López, J. C., Vargas, J. P., Broglio, C., Gómez, Y., & Salas, C. (2002). Spatial memory and hippocampal pallium through vertebrae evolution: Insights from reptiles and teleost fish. Brain Research Bulletin, 57(3/4), 499503.
  • Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701703.
  • Sholl, M. J., & Nolin, T. L. (1997). Orientation specificity in representations of place. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(6), 14941507.
  • Smith, M., & Crawford, J. (2005). Distributed population mechanism for the 3-D oculomotor reference frame transformation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 93, 17421761.
  • Touretzky, D. S., & Redish, A. D. (1996). Theory of rodent navigation based on interacting representations of space. Hippocampus, 6, 247270.
  • Trickett, S. B., & Trafton, J. G. (2007). “What if...’’: The use of conceptual simulations in scientific reasoning. Cognitive Science, 31(5), 843875.
  • Xing, J., & Andersen, R. A. (2000). Models of the posterior parietal cortex which perform multimodal integration and represent space in several different coordinate frames. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(4), 601614.
  • Zipser, D., & Andersen, R. A. (1988). A back-propagation programmed network that simulates response properties of a subset of posterior parietal neurons. Nature, 331, 679684.
  • Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123(2), 162185.