SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Allen, S. (2007). Interacting pragmatic influences on children’s argument realization. In P. M. Brown & M. Bowerman (Eds.), Crosslinguistic perspectives on argument structure: Implications for learnability (pp. 191210). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Anselmi, D., Tomasello, M., & Acunzo, M. (1986). Young children’s responses to neutral and specific contingent queries. Journal of Child Language, 13(1), 135144.
  • Ariel, M. (1988). Referring and accessibility. Journal of Linguistics, 24, 6587.
  • Ariel, M. (1990). Anaphoric antecedents. London: Croom Helm.
  • Arnold, J. E., & Griffin, Z. (2007). The effect of additional characters on choice of referring expression: Everyone counts. Journal of Memory & Language, 56(4), 521536.
  • Arts, A., Maes, A., Noordman, L., & Jansen, C. (2011). Overspecification facilitates object identification. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(1), 361374.
  • Astington, J., & Baird, J. (Eds.) (2005). Why language matters for theory of mind. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390412.
  • Bahtiyar, S., & Küntay, A. C. (2009). Integration of communicative partner’s visual perspective in patterns of referential requests. Journal of Child Language, 36(3), 529555.
  • Balaban, M. T., & Waxman, S. R. (1997). Do words facilitate object categorization in 9-month-old infants? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64(1), 326.
  • Bamberg, M. (1987). The acquisition of narratives: Learning to use language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Bard, E. G., Anderson, A. H., Chen, Y., Nicholson, H. B. M., Havard, C., & Dalzel-Job, S. (2007). Let’s you do that: Sharing the cognitive burdens of dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(4), 616641.
  • Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.
  • Belz, A., & Gatt, A. (2008). Intrinsic vs extrinsic evaluation measures for referring expression generation. Paper presented at the 46th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, Columbus, OH.
  • Brennan, S. E., & Hanna, J. E. (2009). Partner-specific adaptation in dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 274291.
  • Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York: Norton.
  • Campbell, A. L., Brooks, P., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Factors affecting young children’s use of pronouns as referring expressions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(6), 13371349.
  • Clancy, P. (2003). The lexicon in interaction: Developmental origins of preferred argument structure in Korean. In J. Du Bois, L. Kumpf, & W. Ashby (Eds.), Preferred argument structure: Grammar as architecture for function (pp. 81108). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
  • Clark, H. H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition, 22(1), 139.
  • Dale, R., & Reiter, E. (1995). Computational interpretations of the Gricean maxims in the generation of referring expressions. Cognitive Science, 19(2), 233263.
  • Davies, C., & Katsos, N. (2010). Over-informative children: Production/comprehension asymmetry or tolerance to pragmatic violations? Lingua, 120(8), 19561972.
  • Davis, G. A., & Wilcox, M. J. (1985). Adult aphasia rehabilitation: Applied pragmatics. San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.
  • Deutsch, W., & Pechmann, T. (1982). Social interaction and the development of definite descriptions. Cognition, 11, 159184.
  • Dickson, W. P. (1982). Two decades of referential communication research: A review and meta-analysis. In C. J. Brainerd & M. Pressley (Eds.), Verbal processes in children (pp. 133). New York: Springer.
  • Dixon, P. (2008). Models of accuracy in repeated-measures designs. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 447456.
  • Englehardt, P. E., Bailey, K., & Ferreira, F. (2006). Do speakers and listeners observe the Gricean Maxim of Quantity? Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 554573.
  • Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2008). Content or connectedness? Mother-child talk and early social understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 201216.
  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5), 1173, discussion 174–185.
  • Forrester, M. A., & Cherington, S. M. (2009). The development of other-related conversational skills: A case study of conversational repair during the early years. First Language, 29(2), 166191.
  • Fukumura, K., van Gompel, R. P. G., & Pickering, M. (2010). The use of visual context during the production of referring expressions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(9), 17001715.
  • Gallagher, T. (1977). Revision behaviors in the speech of normal children developing language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 20, 303318.
  • Gelman, A., & Hill, J. (2007). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Golinkoff, R. M. (1986). “I beg your pardon?”: The preverbal negociation of failed messages. Journal of Child Language, 13, 455476.
  • Goudbeek, M., & Krahmer, E. (2010). Preferences versus adaptation during referring expression generation. Paper presented at the Association for Compuational Linguistics, Uppsala, Sweden.
  • Greenfield, P. M. (1979). Informativeness, presupposition and semantic choice in single word utterances. In E. Ochs & B. Schieffelin (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics (pp. 159166). New York: Academic Press.
  • Guhe, M., & Bard, E. G. (2008). Adapting referring expressions to the task environment. Paper presented at the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Austin, TX.
  • Heller, D., Skovbroten, K., & Tanenhaus, M. (2009). Experimental evidence for speakers’ sensitivity to common vs. priviledged ground in the production of names. Paper presented at the Workshop on the Production of Referring Expressions (PRE-CogSci), Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
  • Hickmann, M., & Hendriks, H. (1999). Cohesion and anaphora in children’s narratives: A comparison of English, French, German and Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language, 26, 419452.
  • Horton, W. S., & Spieler, D. H. (2007). Age-related differences in communication and audience design. Psychology and Aging, 22(2), 281290.
  • Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434446.
  • Jordan, P., & Walker, M. A. (2005). Learning content selection rules for generating object descriptions in dialogue. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 24, 157194.
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1981). The grammatical marking of thematic structure in the development of language production. In W. Deutsch (Ed.), The child’s construction of language (pp. 121147). London: Academic Press.
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Kloo, D., & Perner, J. (2005). Disentangling dimensions in the dimensional change card-sorting task. Developmental Science, 8(1), 4456.
  • Krahmer, E., & Theune, M. (2002). Efficient context-sensitive generation of referring expressions. In K. van Deemter & R. Kibble (Eds.), Information sharing: Reference and presupposition in language generation and interpretation (pp. 223264). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
  • Krahmer, E., & van der Sluis, I. (2003). A model for generating referring expressions. Paper presented at the 9th European Workshop on Natural Language Generation (ENLG-2003), Budapest, Hungary.
  • Kraljic, T., & Brennan, S. E. (2005). Prosodic disambiguation of syntactic structure: For the speaker or for the addressee? Cognitive Psychology, 50(2), 194231.
  • Küntay, A. C. (2002). Development of the expression of indefiniteness: Presenting new referents in Turkish picture-series stories. Discourse Processes, 33(1), 77101.
  • Küntay, A. C., & Özyürek, A. (2006). Learning to use demonstratives in conversation: What do language specific strategies in Turkish reveal? Journal of Child Language, 33(2), 303320.
  • Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108(3), 732739.
  • Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74(4), 11301144.
  • Matthews, D. E., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents for others: A training study. Child Development, 78(6), 17441759.
  • Matthews, D. E., Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2006). The effect of perceptual availability and prior discourse on young children’s use of referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 403422.
  • Moll, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Fourteen-month-olds know what others experience only in joint engagement with them. Developmental Science, 10(6), 826835.
  • Nadig, A. S., & Sedivy, J. C. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children’s on-line reference resolution. Psychological Science, 13, 329336.
    Direct Link:
  • Namy, L. L., & Gentner, D. (2002). Making a silk purse out of two sows’ ears: Young children’s use of comparison in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 515.
  • Nelson, K., Plesa Skwerer, D., Goldman, S., Henseler, S., Presler, N., & Fried Walkenfeld, F. (2003). Entering a community of minds: An experiential approach to ‘Theory of Mind’. Human Development, 46(1), 2446.
  • Nilsen, E. S., & Graham, S. A. (2009). The relations between children’s communicative perspective-taking and executive functioning. Cognitive Psychology, 58(2), 220249.
  • O’Neill, D. (1996). Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to a parent’s knowledge state when making requests. Child Development, 67, 659677.
  • O’Neill, D., & Topolovec, J. (2001). Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to the referential (in)efficacy of their own pointing gestures. Journal of Child Language, 28, 128.
  • Paraboni, I., van Deemter, K., & Masthoff, J. (2007). Generating referring expressions: Making referents easy to identify. Computational Linguistics, 33(2), 229254.
  • Pechmann, T. (1989). Incremental speech production and referential overspecification. Linguistics, 27, 98110.
  • Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (1999). Representing inner worlds: Theory of mind in autistic, deaf and normal hearing children. Psychological Science, 10(2), 126129.
    Direct Link:
  • Plunkett, K., Hu, J., & Cohen, L. B. (2008). Labels can override perceptual categories in early infancy. Cognition, 106(2), 665681.
  • Pulvermuller, F., Neininger, B., Elbert, T., Mohr, B., Rockstroh, B., Koebbel, P., & Taub, E. (2001). Constraint-induced therapy of chronic aphasia after stroke. Stroke, 32(7), 16211626.
  • Robinson, E. J., & Robinson, W. P. (1985). Teaching children about verbal referential communication. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 8, 285299.
  • Rozendaal, M. I., & Baker, A. E. (2008). A cross-linguistic investigation of the acquisition of the pragmatics of indefinite and definite reference in two-year-olds. Journal of Child Language, 35(04), 773807.
  • Salazar Orvig, A., Marcos, H., Morgenstern, A., Hassan, R., Leber-Marin, J., & Parès, J. (2010). Dialogical beginnings of anaphora: The use of third person pronouns before the age of 3. Journal of Pragmatics, 42(7), 18421865.
  • Salomo, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Young children’s sensitivity to new and given information when answering predicate-focus questions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31(01), 101115.
  • Serratrice, L. (2004). Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax-pragmatics interface: Subjects and objects in English-Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7(3), 183205.
  • Shwe, H., & Markman, E. M. (1997). Young children’s appreciation of the mental impacts of their communicative signals. Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 630636.
  • Skarabela, B. (2007). Signs of early social cognition in children’s syntax: The case of joint attention in argument realization in child Inuktitut. Lingua, 117(11), 18371857.
  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705722.
  • van der Sluis, I., & Krahmer, E. (2007). Generating multimodal references. Discourse Processes, 44(3), 145174.
  • Viethen, J., & Dale, R. (2008). Generating referring expressions: What makes a difference? Paper presented at the Australian Language Technology Association Workshop 2008, Hobart, Australia.
  • Viethen, J., & Dale, R. (2009). Referring expression generation: What can we learn from human data? Paper presented at the Workshop on Production of Referring Expressions: Bridging the Gap Between Computational and Empirical Approaches to Reference, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
  • Viethen, J., Dale, R., Krahmer, E., Theune, M., & Touset, P. (2008). Controlling redundancy in referring expressions. Paper presented at the 6th Language Resources and Evaluation Conference, Marrakesh, Morocco.
  • Walker, M. A. (1996). The effect of resource limits and task complexity on collaborative planning in dialogue. Artificial Intelligence, 85(1–2), 181243.
  • Whitehurst, G., Sonnenschein, S., & Ianfolla, B. (1981). Learning to communicate from models: Children confuse length with information. Child Development, 52, 507513.
  • Wilcox, M. J., & Webster, E. J. (1980). Early discourse behaviour: An analysis of children’s responses to listner feedback. Child Development, 51, 11201125.
  • Wilson, D., & Sperber, D. (2006). Relevance theory. In L. R. Horn & G. Ward (Eds.), The handbook of pragmatics (pp. 607632). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
  • Wittek, A., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Young children’s sensitivity to listener knowledge and perceptual context in choosing referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26(4), 541558.