SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • Annett, J. (1995). Motor imagery: Perception or action?Neuropsychologia, 33(11), 13951417.
  • Arbib, M. A. (1985). Coordinated control programs for movements of the hand. Hand Function and the Neocortex. Experimental Brain Research, 10, 111129.
  • Bonini, L., Rozzi, S., Serventi, F. U., Simone, L., Ferrari, P. F., & Fogassi, L. (2010). Ventral premotor and inferior parietal cortices make distinct contribution to action organization and intention understanding. Cerebral Cortex, 20(6), 13721385.
  • Borroni, P., Gorini, A., Riva, G., Bouchard, S., & Cerri, G. (2011). Mirroring avatars: dissociation of action and intention in human motor resonance. European Journal of Neuroscience, 34(4), 662669.
  • Bratman, M. (1987). Intentions, Plans, and Practical Reasoning. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Brewer, B. (1999). Perception and Reason. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  • Buccino, G., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Rodà, F., & Riggio, L. (2009). Broken affordances, broken objects: a TMS study. Neuropsychologia, 47(14), 30743078. PMID: 19615389.
  • Campbell, J. (2002). Reference and Consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Cardellicchio, P., Sinigaglia, C., & Costantini, M. (2011). The space of affordances: A TMS study. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 13691372.
  • Cattaneo, L., Caruana, F., Jezzini, A., & Rizzolatti, G. (2009). Representation of goal and movements without overt motor behavior in the human motor cortex: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(36), 1113411138.
  • Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., Boria, S., Pieraccini, C., Monti, A., Cossu, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (2007). Impairment of actions chains in autism and its possible role in intention understanding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(45), 1782517830.
  • Cattaneo, L., Sandrini, M., & Schwarzbach, J. (2010). State-Dependent TMS reveals a hierarchical representation of observed acts in the temporal, parietal, and premotor cortices. Cerebral Cortex, 20(9), 22522258.
  • Cavallo, A., Becchio, C., Sartori, L., Bucchioni, G., & Castiello, U. (2011). Grasping with tools: Corticospinal excitability reflects observed hand movements. Cerebral Cortex.
  • Costantini, M., Ambrosini, E., Sinigaglia, C., & Gallese, V. (2011). Tool-use observation makes far objects ready-to-hand. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 26582663.
  • Costantini, M., Ambrosini, E., Tieri, G., Sinigaglia, C., & Committeri, G. (2010). Where does an object trigger an action? an investigation about affordances in space. Experimental Brain Research, 207(1–2), 95103.
  • Currie, G. & Ravenscroft, I. (1997). Mental simulation and motor imagery. Philosophy of Science, 64(1), 161180.
  • Decety, J. (1996). Do imagined and executed actions share the same neural substrate?Cognitive Brain Research, 3(2), 8793.
  • Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Prablanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 34(1–2), 3542.
  • Dokic, J. & Pacherie, E. (2001). Shades and concepts. Analysis, 61(271), 193202.
  • Dretske, F. (1988). Explaining Behavior. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
  • Fiori, F., Sedda, A., Ferrè, E. R., Toraldo, A., Querzola, M., Pasotti, F., Ovadia, D., Piroddi, C., Dell'acquila, R., Lunetta, C., Corbo, M., & Bottini, G. (2012). Exploring motor and visual imagery in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Submitted.
  • Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308(5722), 662667.
  • Fourkas, A. D., Ionta, S., & Aglioti, S. M. (2006). Influence of imagined posture and imagery modality on corticospinal excitability. Behavioural Brain Research, 168(2), 190196.
  • Frak, V., Paulignan, Y., & Jeannerod, M. (2001). Orientation of the opposition axis in mentally simulated grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 136(1), 120127.
  • Gallese, V. (2000). The inner sense of action. agency and motor representations. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7(10), 2340.
  • Gallese, V. & Sinigaglia, C. (2011). Cognition in action: A new look at the motor system. In H. Terrace (Ed.), Joint Attention and Agency. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Gendler, T. (2011). Imagination. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2011 ed.).
  • Gentilucci, M., Daprati, E., & Gangitano, M. (1998). Right-handers and left-handers have different representations of their own hand. Cognitive Brain Research, 6(3), 185192.
  • Heck Jr., R. G. (2000). Nonconceptual content and the ‘space of reasons’. The Philosophical Review, 109(4), 483523.
  • Ishai, A. & Sagi, D. (1995). Common mechanisms of visual imagery and perception. Science, 268(5218), 17721774.
  • Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17(2), 187245.
  • Jeannerod, M. (1998). The Neural and Behavioural Organization of Goal-Directed Movements. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14(1), S103–S109–S103–S109.
  • Jeannerod, M. (2003). The mechanism of self-recognition in humans. Behavioural Brain Research, 142(1–2), 115.
  • Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor Cognition: What Actions Tell the Self. Oxford University Press.
  • Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M. A., Rizzolatti, G., & Sakata, H. (1995). Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends in Neurosciences, 18(7), 314320.
  • Jeannerod, M. & Decety, J. (1995). Mental motor imagery: a window into the representational stages of action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5(6), 727732.
  • Jordan, K., Heinze, H., Lutz, K., Kanowski, M., & Jäncke, L. (2001). Cortical activations during the mental rotation of different visual objects. Neuroimage, 13(1), 143152.
  • Koch, G., Versace, V., Bonnì, S., Lupo, F., Gerfo, E. L., Oliveri, M., & Caltagirone, C. (2010). Resonance of cortico–cortical connections of the motor system with the observation of goal directed grasping movements. Neuropsychologia, 48(12), 35133520.
  • Kosslyn, S., Ball, T., & Reiser, B. (1978). Visual images preserve metric spatial information: Evidence from studies of image scanning. Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance4(1), 47.
  • Kosslyn, S., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 2(9), 635642.
  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1978). Measuring the visual angle of the mind's eye. Cognitive Psychology, 10(3), 356389.
  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1996). Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery Debate. MIT Press.
  • Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Wraga, M., & Alpert, N. M. (2001b). Imagining rotation by endogenous versus exogenous forces: Distinct neural mechanisms. Neuroreport, 12(11), 25192525.
  • Levine, J. (2010). Demonstrative thought. Mind & Language, 25(2), 169195.
  • Linkenauger, S., Witt, J., Stefanucci, J., Bakdash, J., & Proffitt, D. (2009). The effects of handedness and reachability on perceived distance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(6), 1649.
  • Mason, C. R., Gomez, J. E., & Ebner, T. J. (2001). Hand synergies during reach-to-grasp. Journal of Neurophysiology, 86(6), 28962910.
  • McDowell, J. H. (1996). Mind and World. Cambridge, Mass; London: Harvard University Press.
  • Miall, R. & Wolpert, D. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Networks, 9(8), 12651279.
  • Nico, D., Daprati, E., Rigal, F., Parsons, L., & Sirigu, A. (2004). Left and right hand recognition in upper limb amputees. Brain, 127(1), 120132.
  • Pacherie, E. (2000). The content of intentions. Mind and Language, 15(4), 400432.
  • Pacherie, E. (2008). The phenomenology of action: A conceptual framework. Cognition, 107(1), 179217.
  • Page, J. W., Duhamel, P., & Crognale, M. A. (2011). ERP evidence of visualization at early stages of visual processing. Brain and Cognition, 75(2), 141146.
  • Parsons, L. (1987). Imagined spatial transformations of one's hands and feet. Cognitive Psychology, 19(2), 178241.
  • Parsons, L., Gabrieli, J., Phelps, E., & Gazzaniga, M. (1998). Cerebrally lateralized mental representations of hand shape and movement. The Journal of neuroscience, 18(16), 65396548.
  • Parsons, L. M. (1994). Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 709730.
  • Pearson, J., Clifford, C., & Tong, F. (2008). The functional impact of mental imagery on conscious perception. Current Biology, 18(13), 982986.
  • Ramsey, R., Cumming, J., Eastough, D., & Edwards, M. G. (2010). Incongruent imagery interferes with action initiation. Brain and Cognition, 74(3), 249254.
  • Rizzolatti, G., Camarda, R., Fogassi, L., Gentilucci, M., Luppino, G., & Matelli, M. (1988). Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. Experimental Brain Research, 71(3), 491507.
  • Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 2(9), 661670.
  • Rizzolatti, G. & Sinigaglia, C. (2008). Mirrors in the Brain: How Our Minds Share Actions, Emotions. Oxford University Press.
  • Rochat, M. J., Caruana, F., Jezzini, A., Escola, L., Intskirveli, I., Grammont, F., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, M. A. (2010). Responses of mirror neurons in area f5 to hand and tool grasping observation. Experimental Brain Research, 204(4), 605616.
  • Santello, M., Flanders, M., & Soechting, J. F. (2002). Patterns of hand motion during grasping and the influence of sensory guidance. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22(4), 14261435.
  • Sirigu, A. & Duhamel, J. R. (2011). Motor and visual imagery as two complementary but neurally dissociable mental processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(7), 910919.
  • Tucker, M. & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 830846.
  • Tucker, M. & Ellis, R. (2001). The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization. Visual Cognition, 8(6), 769800.
  • Umiltà, M. A., Escola, L., Intskirveli, I., Grammont, F., Rochat, M., Caruana, F., Jezzini, A., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2008). When pliers become fingers in the monkey motor system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(6), 22092213.
  • Umiltà, M. A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2001). I know what you are doing: A neurophysiological study. Neuron, 31(1), 155165.
  • Vargas, C., Olivier, E., Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Duhamel, J., & Sirigu, A. (2004). The influence of hand posture on corticospinal excitability during motor imagery: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cerebral Cortex, 14(11), 12001206.
  • Villiger, M., Chandrasekharan, S., & Welsh, T. N. (2010). Activity of human motor system during action observation is modulated by object presence. Experimental Brain Research, 209, 8593.
  • Witt, J. (2011). Action's effect on perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(3), 201.
  • Witt, J. & Dorsch, T. (2009). Kicking to bigger uprights: Field goal kicking performance influences perceived size. Perception, 38(9), 13281340.
  • Wolpert, D., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269(5232), 18801882.