SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

References

  • American Psychiatric Association (1994). DSM-IV: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248254.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?, Cognition, 21, 3746.
  • Buxhoeveden, B. P., & Casanova, M. F. (2002). The minicolumn hypothesis in neuroscience. Brain, 125, 935951.
  • Casanova, M. F., van Kooten, I. A. J., Switala, A. E., van Engeland, H., Heinsen, H., Steinbusch, H. W. M., Hof, P. R., Trippe, J., Stone, J., & Schmitz, C. (2006). Minicolumnar abnormalities in autism. Acta Neuroathologica, 112, 287303.
  • Dautenhahn, K., Werry, I., Rae, J., Dickerson, P., Stribling, P., & Ogden, B. (2002). Robotic playmates: Analysing interactive competencies of children with autism playing with a mobile robot. In K. Dautenhahn , A. Bond , L. Cañamero , & B. Edmonds (Eds.), Socially intelligent agents: Creating relationships with computers and robots. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers, pp. 117124.
  • Dennett, D. C. (1989). The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma. 2nd ed. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
  • Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naïve theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287292.
  • Herbert, M. R. (2005). Large brains in autism: The challenge of pervasive abnormality. Neuroscientist, 11, 417440.
  • Kozima, H. (2011). Microscopic predictability and macroscopic predictability: Comments on Takahashi and Miyazaki's article. Japanese Psychological Review, 54, 2528. (In Japanese.).
  • Kozima, H., Michalowski, M. P., & Nakagawa, C. (2009). Keepon: A playful robot for research, therapy, and entertainment. International Journal of Social Robotics, 1, 318.
  • Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R. L., & Schreibman, L. (1979). Stimulus overselectivity in autism: A review of research. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 12361254.
  • Minsky, M. L. (1980). K-lines: A theory of memory. Cognitive Science, 4, 117133.
  • Minsky, M. L. (1986). Society of mind. New York: Simon and Schuster.
  • Norman, D. A. (1988). The psychology of everyday things. New York: Basic Books.
  • Reeves, B., & Nass, C. (1996). The media equation: How people treat computers, television, and new media like real people and places. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  • Rinkus, G. J. (2010). A cortical sparse distributed coding model linking mini- and macrocolumn-scale functionality. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, 4, Article 17, 113.
  • Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representation of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192233.
  • Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382439.
  • Takahashi, H., & Miyazaki, M. (2011). Neural mechanism for top-down preparation dependent on self, other, physical object and its uniqueness in autism. Japanese Psychological Review, 54, 624. (In Japanese).
  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Wing, L. (1981). Language, social and cognitive impairments in autism and severe mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10, 3144.