SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

REFERENCES

  • Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 170177. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010
  • Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2005). Unified segmentation. NeuroImage, 26, 839851. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018
  • Babai, R., & Amsterdamer, A. (2008). The persistence of solid and liquid naive conceptions: A reaction time study. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 17, 553559. doi:10.1007/s10956-008-9122-6
  • Babai, R., Eidelman, R., & Stavy, R. (2012). Preactivation of inhibitory control mechanisms hinders intuitive reasoning. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 10, 763775. doi:10.1007/s10763-011-9287-y
  • Bachelard, G. (1938). La formation de l'esprit scientifique. Paris, France: Vrin.
  • Bandettini, P. A., & Cox, R. W. (2000). Event-related fMRI contrast when using constant interstimulus interval: Theory and experiment. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 43, 540548.
  • Bélanger, M.. (2008). Du changement conceptuel à la complexification conceptuelle dans l'apprentissage des sciences (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Université de Montréal, Canada.
  • Blanke, O., Mohr, C., Michel, C. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Brugger, P., Seeck, M., … Thut, G. (2005). Linking out-of-body experience and self processing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 550557. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2612-04.2005
  • Botvinick, M. M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavior Neuroscience, 7, 356366.
  • Brown, D. E. (1993). Refocusing core intuitions: A concretizing role for analogy in conceptual change. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 20, 12731290.
  • Buckner, R. L. (1998). Event-related fMRI and the hemodynamic response. Human Brain Mapping, 6, 373377.
  • Bush, G., Whalen, P. J., Rosen, B. R., Jenike, M. A., McInerney, S. C., & Rauch, S. L. (1998). The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging–validation study with functional MRI. Human Brain Mapping, 6, 270282.
  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Çepni, S., & Keleş, E. (2006). Turkish students' conceptions about the simple electric circuits. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 4, 269291.
  • Chen, C.-Y., Muggleton, N. G., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., & Juan, C.-H. (2009). Control of prepotent responses by the superior medial frontal cortex. NeuroImage, 44, 537545.
  • Chi, M. T. H. (1994). From things to processes: A theory of conceptual change for learning science concepts. Learning and Instruction, 4, 2743.
  • De Neys, W., Vartanian, O., & Goel, V. (2008). Smarter than we think: When our brains detect that we are biased. Psychological Science, 19, 483489. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02113.x
    Direct Link:
  • Duit, R., & Treagust, D. F. (2003). Conceptual change: A powerful framework for improving science teaching and learning. International Journal of Science Education, 25, 671688.
  • Duit, R., & Treagust, D. F. (2012). How can conceptual change contribute to theory and practice in science education? In B. J. Fraser, K. Tobin & C. J. McRobbie (Eds.), Second international handbook of science education (pp. 107118). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.
  • Dunbar, K. N., Fugelsang, J. A., & Stein, C. (2007). Do naïve theories ever go away? Using brain and behavior to understand changes in concepts. In M. C. Lovett & P. Shah (Eds.), Thinking with data: 33rd Carnegie symposium on cognition (pp. 193206). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Friston, K. J., Williams, S., Howard, R., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Turner, R. (1996). Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 35, 346355.
  • Giordan, A., & DeVecchi, G. (1987). Les origines du savoir. Neuchatel, Switzerland: Delachaux.
  • Goldberg, R. F., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009). Developmental “roots” in mature biological knowledge. Psychological Science, 20, 480487. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02320.x
    Direct Link:
  • Grabner, R. H., Ansari, D., Reishofer, G., Stern, E., Ebner, F., & Neuper, C. (2007). Individual differences in mathematical competence predict parietal brain activation during mental calculation. NeuroImage, 38, 346356. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.041
  • Houdé, O., Zago, L., Crivello, F., Moutier, S., Pineau, A., Mazoyer, B., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2001). Access to deductive logic depends on a right ventromedial prefrontal area devoted to emotion and feeling: Evidence from a training paradigm. NeuroImage, 14, 14861492.
  • Houdé, O., Zago, L., Mellet, E., Moutier, S., Pineau, A., Mazoyer, B., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2000). Shifting from the perceptual brain to the logical brain: The neural impact of cognitive inhibition training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 721728.
  • Kang, H., Scharmann, L. C., Kang, S., & Noh, T. (2010). Cognitive conflict and situational interest as factors influencing conceptual change. International Journal of Environmental and Science Education, 5, 383405.
  • Kelemen, D., Rottman, J., & Seston, R. (2012). Professional physical scientists display tenacious teleological tendencies: Purpose-based reasoning as a cognitive default. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 10741083. doi:10.1037/a0030399
  • Kwon, Y.-J., & Lawson, A. E. (2000). Linking brain growth with the development of scientific reasoning ability and conceptual change during adolescence. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37, 4462.
  • Liddle, P. F., Kiehl, K. A., & Smith, A. M. (2001). Event-related fMRI study of response inhibition. Human Brain Mapping, 12, 100109.
  • Limón, M. (2001). On the cognitive conflict as an instructional strategy for conceptual change: A critical appraisal. Learning and Instruction, 11, 357380. doi:10.1016/s0959-4752(00)00037-2
  • Liu, X. (2001). Synthesizing research on student conceptions in science. International Journal of Science Education, 23, 5581. doi:10.1080/09500690119778
  • Lombrozo, T., Kelemen, D., & Zaitchik, D. (2007). Inferring design: Evidence of a preference for teleological explanations in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Psychological Science, 18, 9991006. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02015.x
    Direct Link:
  • MacDonald, A. W. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 18351838. doi:10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
  • Masson, S., Potvin, P., Riopel, M., Brault Foisy, L.-M., & Lafortune, S. (2012). Using fMRI to study conceptual change: Why and how? International Journal of Environmental and Science Education, 7, 1935.
  • Menon, V., Adleman, N. E., White, C. D., Glover, G. H., & Reiss, A. L. (2001). Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Human Brain Mapping, 12, 131143.
  • Mitchell, J. P. (2007). Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind. Cerebral cortex, 18, 262271. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm051
  • Monchi, O., Petrides, M., Petre, V., Worsley, K., & Dagher, A. (2001). Wisconsin card sorting revisited: Distinct neural circuits participating in different stages of the task identified by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 77337741.
  • Mortimer, E. F. (1995). Conceptual change or conceptual profile change? Science and Education, 4, 267285. doi:10.1007/bf00486624
  • Nathaniel-James, D. A., Fletcher, P., & Frith, C. D. (1997). The functional anatomy of verbal initiation and suppression using the Hayling Test. Neuropsychologia, 35, 559566. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00104-2
  • Nussbaum, J., & Novick, S. (1982). Alternative frameworks, conceptual conflict and accommodation: Toward a principled teaching strategy. Instructional Science, 11, 183200.
  • Periago, M. C., & Bohigas, X. (2005). A study of second-year engineering students' alternative conceptions about electric potential, current intensity and Ohm's law. European Journal of Engineering Education, 30, 7180.
  • Posner, G. J., Strike, K. A., Hewson, P. W., & Gertzog, W. A. (1982). Accommodation of a scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change. Science Education, 66, 211227.
  • Rubia, K., Russell, T., Overmeyer, S., Brammer, M. J., Bullmore, E. T., Sharma, T., … Taylor, E. (2001). Mapping motor inhibition: Conjunctive brain activations across different versions of go/no-go and stop tasks. NeuroImage, 13, 250261. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0685
  • Saxe, R. R., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Scholz, J., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2009). Brain regions for perceiving and reasoning about other people in school-aged children. Child Development, 80, 11971209. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01325.x
  • diSessa, A. A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10, 105225.
  • diSessa, A. A. (2006). A history of conceptual change research: Threads and fault lines. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 265281). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Shtulman, A., & Valcarcel, J. (2012). Scientific knowledge suppresses but does not supplant earlier intuitions. Cognition, 124, 209215. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.04.005
  • Smith, C. L. (2007). Bootstrapping processes in the development of students' commonsense matter theories: Using analogical mappings, thought experiments, and learning to measure to promote conceptual restructuring. Cognition and Instruction, 25, 337398. doi:10.1080/07370000701632363
  • Solomon, J. (1983). Learning about energy: How pupils think in two domains. European Journal of Science Education, 5, 4959.
  • Solomon, J. (1984). Prompts, cues and discrimination: The utilization of two separate knowledge systems. European Journal of Science Education, 6, 277284.
  • Stavy, R., Babai, R., Tsamir, P., Tirosh, D., Lin, F.-L., & McRobbie, C. (2006). Are intuitive rules universal? International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 4, 417436.
  • Stavy, R., & Tirosh, D., (2000). How students (mis-)understand science and mathematics: Intuitive rules. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
  • Vosniadou, S. (1994). Capturing and modeling the process of conceptual change. Learning and Instruction, 4, 4569. doi:10.1016/0959-4752(94)90018-3
  • Vosniadou, S. (2008). Conceptual change research: An introduction. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. xiiixxviii). New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Vosniadou, S. (2012). Reframing the classical approach to conceptual change: Preconceptions, misconceptions and synthetic models. In B. J. Fraser, K. Tobin, & C. J. McRobbie (Eds.), Second international handbook of science education (pp. 119130). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.
  • Wandersee, J. H., Mintzes, J. J., & Novak, J. D. (1994). Research on alternative conceptions in science. In D. L. Gabel (Ed.), Handbook of research on science teaching and learning (pp. 177210). New York, NY: Macmillan.
  • Zaitchik, D., & Solomon, G. E. (2008). Animist thinking in the elderly and in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 2737. doi:10.1080/02643290801904059