SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

REFERENCES

  • Baillargeon, R. (1986). Representing the existence and the location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6- and 8-month-old infants. Cognition, 23, 2141.
  • Berthental, B.I., Proffitt, D.R., Spertner, N.B., & Thomas, M.A. (1985). The development of infant sensitivity to biomechanical motions. Child Development, 56, 531543.
  • Briars, D.J., & Siegler, R.S. (1984). A featural analysis of preschoolers' counting knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 20, 607618.
  • Brunswick, E. (1952). The conceptual framework of psychology. International Encyclopedia of Social Science, Vol. 1, Chicago : University of Chicago Press.
  • Bullock, M. (1985). Animism in childhood thinking: A new look at an old question. Developmental Psychology, 21, 217225.
  • Bullock, M., Gelman, R., & Baillargeon, R. (1982). The development of causal reasoning. In W.J. Friedman (Ed.), The developmental psychology of time. New York : Academic.
  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge , MA : MIT Press/Bradford Books.
  • Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C.R. (1984). Testing the geometric power of an animal's spatial representation. In H.L. Roitblatt, T.G. Bever, & H.S. Terrace (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp. 409423). Hillsdale , NJ : Erlbaum.
  • Cooper, R.G., Jr. (1984). Early number development: Discovering number space with addition and subtraction. In C. Sophian (Ed.), The origins of cognitive skills (pp. 157192). Hillsdale , NJ : Erlbaum.
  • Fuson, K.C. (1988). Children's counting and concepts of number. New York : Springer-Verlag.
  • Gallistel, C.R. (1980). The organization of action: A new synthesis. Hillsdale , NJ : Erlbaum.
  • Gallistel, C.R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge , MA : MIT/Bradford Press.
  • Garcia, J., & Koelling, R.A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science, 4, 123124.
  • Gelman, R., Cohen, M., & Hartnett, P. (1989). To understand number is to go beyond the belief that “numbers are what you get when you count.” In Proceedings of the Eleventh Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter, International Group for Psychology of Mathematics Education. September, 1989. Rutgers University, New Brunswick , NJ .
  • Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C.R. (1978). The child's understanding of number. Cambridge , MA : Harvard University Press.
  • Gelman, R., & Greeno, J.G. (1989). On the nature of competence: Principles for understanding in a domain. In L.B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing and learning: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 125186). Hillsdale , NJ : Erlbaum.
  • Gelman, R., Spelke, E.S., & Meek, E. (1983). What preschoolers know about animate and inanimate objects. In D. Rogers & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), The acquisition of symbolic skills. London : Plenum.
  • Golinkoff, R.M., & Harding, C.G. (1980, March). Infants expectations of the movement potential of inanimate objects. Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, New Haven , CT .
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Inhelder, B. (197475). If you want to get ahead, get a theory. Cognition, 3, 195212.
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986). From meta-processes to conscious access: Evidence from children's metalinguistic and repair data. Cognition, 23, 95147.
  • Keil, F.C. (1979). Semantic and conceptual development. Cambridge , MA : Harvard University Press.
  • Keil, F.C. (1987). Conceptual development and category structure. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization. (pp. 175200) New York : Cambridge University Press.
  • Knudsen, E. (1983). Early auditory experience aligns the auditory map of space in the optic tectum of the barn owl. Science, 222, 939942.
  • Landau, B., Smith, L.B., & Jones, S.S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3, 299321.
  • Leslie, A., & Keeble, S. (1987). Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition, 25, 265288.
  • Mandler, J.M. (1988). How to make a baby: On the development of an accessible representational system. Cognitive Development, 3, 113136.
  • Mandler, J.M., Bauer, P.J., & McDonough, L. (1989). Separating the sheep from the goats: Differentiating global categories. Submitted for review. University of California, San Diego , June, 1989.
  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco : W.W. Freeman.
  • Massey, C. (1988). Development of the animate-inanimate distinction in preschoolers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia .
  • Massey, C., & Gelman, R. (1988). Preschoolers' ability to decide whether pictured unfamiliar objects can move themselves. Developmental Psychology, 24, 307317.
  • Nelson, K. (1988). Constraints on word learning? Cognitive Development, 3, 221246.
  • Poulin-Dubois, D., & Shultz, T.R. (1986). The infant's concept of agency: The distinction between animate and inanimate objects. Unpublished manuscript, Concordia University/McGill University, Montreal , Canada .
  • Rescorla, R., & Wagner, A. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement. In A.H. Black & W.F. Prokasy, (Eds.), Classical conditioning. II. New York : Appleton-Century Crofts.
  • Richards, D.D., & Siegler, R.S. (1986). Children's understanding of the attributes of life. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 122.
  • Rozin, P., & Schull, J. (1988). The adaptive-evolutionary point of view in experimental psychology. In R.C. Atkinson, R.J. Hernstein, G. Lindzey, & R.D. Luce (Eds.), Handbook of experimental psychology. (pp. 503546) New York : Wiley-Interscience.
  • Saxe, G.B., Guberman, S.R., & Gearhart, M. (1988). Social processes in early development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 52 (Serial No. 216).
  • Smith, L.B. (1989, April). In defense of perceptual similarity. Paper presented at the Bi-Annual Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City , MO .
  • Sophian, C., & Adams, N. (1987). Infants' understanding of numerical transformations. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 257264.
    Direct Link:
  • Sophian, C., & Huber, A. (1984). Early developments in children's causal judgments. Child Development, 55, 512526.
  • Shipley, E., & Spelke, E.S. (1988, November). Ostensive definition. Unpublished manuscript. University of Pennsylvania/Cornell University.
  • Spelke, E.S. (1985). Preferential looking methods as tools for the study of cognition in infancy. In G. Gottlieb, & N. Krasnegor (Eds.), Measurement of audition and vision in the first year of postnatal life. (pp. 323363). Norwood , NJ : Ablex.
  • Starkey, P.D., German, R., & Spelke, E.S. (1985). Response to Davis, Albert & Baron's Detection of number or numerousness by human infants. Science, 288, 12221223.
  • Starkey, P.D., Spelke, E.S., & Gelman, R. (1983). Detection of intermodal correspondences by human infants. Science, 222, 179181.
  • Strauss, M.S., & Curtis, L.E. (1984). Development of numerical concepts in infancy. In C. Sophian (Ed.), The origins of cognitive skills. Hillsdale , NJ : Erlbaum.