SEARCH

SEARCH BY CITATION

REFERENCES

  • Abler, W. (1989). On the particulate principle of self-diversifying systems. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 12, 113.
  • Abler, W. L. (1997). Gene, language, number: The particulate principle in nature. Evolutionary Theory, 11, 237248.
  • Aslin, R. N., Saffran, J. R., & Newport, E. L. (1998). Computation of conditional probability statistics by 8-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 9, 321324.
    Direct Link:
  • Baldwin, D. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language acquisition. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 133156). Hillsdale : Erlbaum.
  • Bickerton, D. (1990). Species and language. Chicago : Chicago University Press.
  • Bloom, P. (1994). Language acquisition: Core readings. Cambridge : MIT Press.
  • Bloom, P., & Markson, L. (1998). Capacities underlying word learning. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 6773.
  • Borden, G. J., & Harris, K. S. (1984). Speech science primer: Physiology, acoustics, and perception of speech. Baltimore : Williams & Wilkins.
  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), Serial 255, 1174.
  • Catchpole, C. K., & Slater, P. (1995). Bird song: Biological themes and variations. Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.
  • Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1980). Vocal recognition in free-ranging vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 28, 362367.
  • Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1988). Assessment of meaning and the detection of unreliable signals by vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 36, 477486.
  • Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990a). How monkeys see the world: Inside the mind of another species. Chicago : Chicago University Press.
  • Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990b). The representation of social relations by monkeys. Cognition, 37, 167196.
  • Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., & Silk, J. (1995). The responses of female baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus) to anomalous social interactions: Evidence for causal reasoning? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109, 134141.
  • Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin, and use. New York : Praeger.
  • Clark, A. P. (1993). Rank differences in the production of vocalizations by wild chimpanzees as a function of social context. American Journal of Primatology, 31, 159180.
  • Cleveland, J., & Snowdon, C. T. (1981). The complex vocal repertoire of the adult cotton-top tamarin. Saguinus oedipus oedipus. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 58, 231270.
  • Cutler, A., & Butterfield, S. (1992). Rhythmic cues to speech segmentation: evidence from juncture misperception. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 218236.
  • Cynx, J. (1990). Experimental determination of a unit of song production in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 310.
  • Dawkins, R., & Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal signals: Information or manipulation. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.) Behavioural ecology (pp. 282309). Oxford : Blackwell Scientific Publications.
  • Doupe, A., & Konishi, M. (1991). Song-selective auditory circuits in the vocal control system of the Zebra Finch. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 88, 1133911343.
  • Evans, C. S., & Marler, P. (1995). Language and animal communication: Parallels and contrasts. In H. Roitblatt & J.-A. Meyer (Eds.), Comparative approaches to cognitive science (pp. 241282). Cambridge , MA : MIT Press.
  • Ficken, M. S., & Popp, J. W. (1992). Syntactical organization of the gargle vocalization of the black-capped chickadee, Parus atricapillus. Ethology, 91, 156168.
  • Fischer, J. (1990). Barbary macaques categorize shrill barks into two call types. Animal Behaviour, 55, 799807.
  • Fitch: W. T. (1997). Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in rhesus macaques. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 12131222.
  • Fitch, W. T., & Hauser, M. D. (1995). Vocal production in nonhuman primates: acoustics, physiology and functional constraints on honest advertisement. American Journal of Primatology, 37, 191219.
  • FitzGibbon, C. D. & Fanshawe, J. W. (1988). Stotting in Thompson's gazelles: an honest signal of condition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 23, 6974.
  • Fodor, J. A. (1994). The elm and the expert. Cambridge : MIT Press.
  • Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge : MIT Press.
  • Gardner, R. A., Gardner, B. T., & Van Cantfort, E. (Eds.). (1989). Teaching sign language to chimpanzees. Albany : State University of New York Press.
  • Gelfer, C. E., Harris, K. S., & Baer, T. (1987). Controlled variables in sentence intonation. In T. Baer, C. Sasaki & K. Harris (Eds.), Laryngeal function in phonation and respiration (pp. 422435). Boston : College-Hill Publishers.
  • Gould, J. L., & Gould, C. G. (1988). The honey bee. New York : Freeman Press.
  • Gouzoules, S., Gouzoules, H., & Marler, P. (1984). Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) screams: Representational signaling in the recruitment of agonistic aid. Animal Behaviour, 32, 182193.
  • Grafen, A. (1990). Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 144, 475546.
  • Green, S. (1975). Variation of vocal pattern with social situation in the Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata): a field study. In L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), Primate behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 1102). New York : Academic Press.
  • Hailman, J. P., & Ficken, M. S. (1987). Combinatorial animal communication with computable syntax: Chick-a-dee calling qualifies as ‘language’ by structural linguistics. Animal Behaviour, 34, 18991901.
  • Hailman, J. P., Ficken, M. S., & Ficken, R. W. (1987). Constraints on the structure of combinatorial “chick-a-dee” calls. Ethology, 75, 6280.
  • Hammerschmidt, K., & Fischer, J. (1997). The vocal repertoire of Barbary macaques: A quantitative analysis of a graded signal system. Ethology, 132, 381399.
  • Harcourt, A. H., Stewart, K. & Hauser, M. D. (1993). The social use of vocalizations by gorillas. I. Social behaviour and vocal repertoire. Behaviour, 124, 89122.
  • Hauser, M. D. (1993a). Do vervet monkey infants cry wolf? Animal Behaviour, 45, 12421244.
  • Hauser, M. D. (1993b). Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) copulation calls: Honest signals for female choice? Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, 254, 9396.
  • Hauser, M. D. (1996). The evolution of communication. Cambridge : MIT Press.
  • Hauser: M. D. (1997). Artifactual kinds and functional design features: What a primate understands without language. Cognition, 64, 285308.
  • Hauser, M. D. (1998a). Expectations about object motion and destination: Experiments with a nonhuman primate. Developmental Science, 1, 3138.
  • Hauser, M. D. (1998b). Functional referents and acoustic similarity: Field playback experiments with rhesus monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 55, 16471658.
  • Hauser, M. D., Agnetta, B., & Perez, C. (1998). Orienting asymmetries in rhesus monkeys: The effect of time-domain changes on acoustic perception. Animal Behaviour, 56, 4147.
  • Hauser, M. D. (1999). The sound and the fury: Primate vocalizations of thought and emotion. In N. Wahlin & B. Merker (Eds.) The origins of music (pp. 77102). Cambridge , UK : Cambridge University Press.
  • Hauser, M. D., & Andersson, K. (1994). Left hemisphere dominance for processing vocalizations in adult, but not infant rhesus monkeys: Field experiments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 91, 39463948.
  • Hauser, M. D., & Fowler, C. (1992). Declination in fundamental frequency is not unique to human speech: evidence from nonhuman primates. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 91, 363369.
  • Hauser, M. D., MacNeilage, P., & Ware, M. (1996). Numerical representations in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93, 15141517.
  • Hauser, M. D., & Marler, P. (1993a). Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). I. Socioecological factors influencing call production. Behavioral Ecology, 4, 194205.
  • Hauser, M. D., & Marler, P. (1993b). Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). II. Costs and benefits of call production and suppression. Behavioral Ecology, 4, 206212.
  • Heffner, H. E., & Heffner, R. S. (1984). Temporal lobe lesions and perception of species-specific vocalizations by macaques. Science, 226, 7576.
  • Herman, L. M., Pack, A. A. & Palmer, M.-S. (1993). Representational and conceptual skills of dolphins. In H. L. Roitblat, L. M. Herman, and P. E. Nachtigall (Eds.), Language and communication: Comparative perspectives (pp. 403442). Hillsdale , NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
  • Hinde, R. A. (1981). Animal signals: Ethological and games-theory approaches are not incompatible. Animal Behaviour, 29, 535542.
  • Hopp, S. L., Sinnott, J. M., Owren, M. J., & Petersen, M. R. (1992). Differential sensitivity of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) and humans (Homo sapiens) to peak position along a synthetic coo call continuum. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 128136.
  • Krebs, J. R., & Dawkins, R. (1984). Animal signals: Mind-reading and manipulation. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural Ecology (pp. 380402). Sunderland : Sinauer Associates Inc.
  • Kroodsma, D. E., & Miller, E. H. (1996). Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Ithaca , NY : Comstock Publishing.
  • Lambrechts, M. M. (1992). Male quality and playback in the great tit. In P. K. McGregor (Ed.), Playback and studies of animal communication (pp. 135152). New York : Plenum Press.
  • Lieberman, P. (1984). The biology and evolution of language. Cambridge : Harvard University Press.
  • Lieberman, P., & Blumstein, S. E. (1988). Speech physiology, speech perception, and acoustic phonetics. Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.
  • Lorenz, K. (1966). Evolution of ritualization in the biological and cultural spheres. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 251, 273284.
  • Macedonia, J. (1991). What is communicated in the antipredator calls of lemurs: Evidence from playback experiments with ring-tailed and ruffed lemurs. Ethology, 86, 177190.
  • Maeda, T., & Masataka, N. (1987). Locale-specific vocal behaviour of the tamarin (Saguinus 1. labiatus). Ethology, 75, 2530.
  • Margoliash, D., & Fortune, E. (1992). Temporal and harmonic combination-sensitive neurons in the Zebra Finch's HVC. Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 43094326.
  • Markman, E. M. (1990). Constraints children place on word meanings. Cognitive Science, 14, 5777.
  • Markman, E. M., & Hutchinson, J. E. (1984). Children's sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relations. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 127.
  • Marler, P. (1977). The structure of animal communication sounds. In T. H. Bullock (Eds.), Recognition of complex acoustic signals (pp. 1735). Berlin : Springer-Verlag.
  • Marler, P. (1985). Representational vocal signals of primates. In B. Hölldobler and M. Lindauer (Eds.), Experimental behavioral ecology and sociobiology (pp. 211221). Stuttgart , Germany : Gustav Fischer Verlag.
  • Marler, P. (1997). Three models of song learning: Evidence from behavior. Journal of Neurobiology, 33, 116.
  • Marler, P., Dufty, A., & Pickert, R. (1986a). Vocal communication in the domestic chicken. I. Does a sender communicate information about the quality of a food referent to a receiver? Animal Behaviour, 34, 188193.
  • Marler, P., Dufty, A., & Pickert, R. (1986b). Vocal communication in the domestic chicken. II. Is a sender sensitive to the presence and nature of a receiver? Animal Behaviour, 34, 194198.
  • Marler, P., Evans, C. S., & Hauser, M. D. (1992). Animal signals? Reference, motivation or both? In H. Papoucek, U. Jürgens, and M. Papoucek (Eds.), Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental approaches (pp. 6686). Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.
  • Masataka, N. (1988). The response of red-chested moustached tamarins to long calls from their natal and alien populations. Animal Behaviour, 36, 5561.
  • Masataka, N., & Fujita, K. (1989). Vocal learning of Japanese and rhesus monkeys. Behaviour, 109, 191199.
  • May, B., Moody, D. B., & Stebbins, W. C. (1989). Categorical perception of conspecific communication sounds by Japanese macaques. Macaca fuscata. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 85, 837847.
  • Maynard Smith, J. (1974). The theory of games and the evolution of animal conflicts. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 47, 209221.
  • Mitani, J., & Marler, P. (1989). A phonological analysis of male gibbon singing behavior. Behaviour, 109, 2045.
  • Mitani, J. C., Hasegawa, T., Gros-Louis, J., Marler, P., & Byrne, R. (1992). Dialects in wild chimpanzees? American Journal of Primatology, 27, 233244.
  • Møller, A. P. (1993). Morphology and sexual selection in the barn swallow Hirundo rustica in Chernobyl, Ukraine. Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, 252, 5157.
  • Nelson, D. A. (1992). Song overproduction, song matching and selective attrition during development. In P. K. McGregor (Ed.), Playback and studies of animal communication (pp. 121134). New York : Plenum Press.
  • Owren, M. J., & Rendall, D. (1998). An affect-conditioning model of nonhuman primate vocal signaling. In D. Owings, M. Beecher & N. Thompson (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology, Vol. 12: Communication (pp. 299346). New York : Plenum Press.
  • Owren, M. J., Dieter, J. A., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1992). ‘Food’ calls produced by adult female rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques, their normally -raised offspring, and offspring cross-fostered between species. Behaviour, 120, 218231.
  • Owren, M. J., Dieter, J. A., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1993). Vocalizations of rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques cross-fostered between species show evidence of only limited modification. Developmental Psychobiology, 26, 389406.
  • Owren, M. J., Seyfarth, R. M. & Cheney, D. L. (1997). The acoustic features of vowel-like grunt calls in chacma baboons (Papio cyncephalus ursinus): Implications for production processes and function. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, 29512963.
  • Pallier, C., Christophe, A., & Mehler, J. (1997). Language-specific listening. Trends in Cognitive Science, 1, 129132.
  • Petersen, M. R., Beecher, M. D., Zoloth, S. R., Moody, D. B., & Stebbins, W. C. (1978). Neural lateralization of species-specific vocalizations by Japanese macaques. Science, 202, 324326.
  • Pierrehumbert, J. (1979). The perception of fundamental frequency declination. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 66, 363369.
  • Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York : William Morrow and Company, Inc.
  • Premack, D. (1986). Gavagai! or the future history of the animal language controversy. Cambridge : MIT Press.
  • Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1995). Origins of human social competence. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 205218). Cambridge : MIT Press.
  • Putnam, H. (1975). Mind, language and reality. Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.
  • Quine, W. V. (1973). On the reasons for the indeterminacy of translation. Journal of Philosophy, 12, 178183.
  • Rendall, D., Rodman, P. S., & Edmond, R. E. (1996). Vocal recognition of individuals and kin in free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 51, 10071015.
  • Robinson, J. G. (1979). An analysis of the organization of vocal communication in the titi monkey Callicebus moloch. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 49, 381405.
  • Robinson, J. G. (1984). Syntactic structures in the vocalizations of wedge-capped capuchin monkeys. Cebus nigrivittatus. Behaviour, 90, 4679.
  • Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1986). Ape language: From conditioned response to symbol. New York : Columbia University Press.
  • Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Murphy, J., Sevcik, R. A., Brakke, K. E., Williams, S. L., & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1993). Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 1221.
  • Schusterman, R. J., Gisiner, R., Grimm, B. K., & Hanggi, E. B. (1993). Behavior control by exclusion and attempts at establishing semanticity in marine mammals using match-to-sample paradigms. In H. L. Roitblat, L. M. Herman, and P. E. Nachtigall (Eds.), Language and communication: Comparative perspectives (pp. 249274). Hillsdale , N. J. : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Searcy, W. A. (1992). Measuring responses of female birds to male song. In P. K. McGregor (Ed.), Playback and studies of animal communication (pp. 175190). New York : Plenum Press.
  • Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1980). The ontogeny of vervet monkey alarm-calling behavior: A preliminary report. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 54, 3756.
  • Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1986). Vocal development in vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 34, 16401658.
  • Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1997). Some general features of vocal development. in nonhuman primates. In C. T. Snowdon and M. Hausberger (Eds.), Social influences on vocal development (pp. 249273). Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.
  • Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., & Marler, P. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: Evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science, 210, 801803.
  • Shettleworth, S. (1998). Cognition, evolution and behavior. New York : Oxford University Press.
  • Smith, W. J. (1977). The behavior of communicating. Cambridge : Harvard University Press.
  • Snowdon, C. T., & Hausberger, M. (1997). Social influences on vocal development. Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.
  • Soja, N., Carey, S., & Spelke, E. S. (1991). Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: object terms and substance terms. Cognition, 38, 179211.
  • Stebbins, W. C., & Sommers, M. S. (1992). Evolution, perception and the comparative method. In D. B. Webster, R. F. Fay, & A. N. Popper (Eds.), The evolutionary biology of hearing (pp. 211228). New York : Springer-Verlag.
  • Struhsaker, T. T. (1967). Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). In S. A. Altmann (Eds.) Social communication among primates (pp. 281324). Chicago : Chicago University Press.
  • Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1998). The particulate origins of language generativity: From syllable to gesture. In J. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy & C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language: Social and cognitive bases (pp. 202221). Cambridge , UK : Cambridge University Press.
  • Thompson, R. K. R. (1995). Natural and relational concepts in animals. In H. L. Roitblat & J. A. Meyer (Eds.), Comparative approaches to cognitive science (pp. 175224). Cambridge : MIT Press.
  • Tinkelpaugh, O. L. (1928). An experimental study of representative factors in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 8, 197236.
  • Todt, D., Hammerschmidt, K., Ansorge, V., & Fischer, J. (1995). The vocal behavior of barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus): Call features and their performance in infants and adult. In E. Zimmerman, J. D. Newman & U. Jurgens (Eds.) Current topics in primate vocal communication (pp. 141160). New York : Plenum Press.
  • Tomasello, M., Call, J., Nagell, K., Olguin, R., & Carpenter, M. (1994). The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: A trans-generational study. Primates, 35, 137154.
  • von Frisch, K. (1967). The dance language and orientation of bees. Cambridge : Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
  • Waser, P. M. (1977). Experimental playbacks show vocal mediation of avoidance on a forest monkey. Nature, 255, 5658.
  • Watanabe, M. (1996). Reward expectancy in primate prefrontal neurons. Nature, 382, 629632.
  • Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection: A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205214.
  • Zahavi, A. (1993). The fallacy of conventional signalling. Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, 340, 227230.
  • Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin's puzzle. Oxford : Oxford University Press.
  • Zoloth, S. R., Petersen, M. R., Beecher, M. D., Green, S., Marler, P., Moody, D. B., & Stebbins, W. C. (1979). Species-specific perceptual processing of vocal sounds by monkeys. Science, 204, 870872.
  • Zuberbuhler, K., Noe, R., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1997). Diana monkey long-distance calls: Messages for conspecifics and predators. Animal Behaviour, 53, 589604.